Based on content analysis, the results of the review are organized in the following four main subsections: (1) mindfulness and autobiographical memory specificity; (2) mindfulness and emotional autobiographical recall; (3) self-inquiry into negative autobiographical narratives and mindfulness; and (4) mindfulness and flashbacks.
Mindfulness and Autobiographical Memory Specificity
How people recall personal memories can be specific or overgeneral. Overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) refers to an inability to retrieve specific memories for events that occurred at a particular time and place, and lasted less than a day, from one’s AM (Williams & Broadbent,
1986). Individuals who are more likely to exhibit this cognitive phenomenon, when asked to come up with a specific autobiographical memory in response to a cue word (positive, negative, or neutral), often retrieve memories that are summaries or classes of events (categorical memories, e.g., “When I am on holiday”), or memories for events that lasted longer than a day (extended memories, e.g., “Last summer”). These two ways of recalling memories have important effects on psychological functioning. For example, difficulties in shifting fluently from general information higher in the AM hierarchy (according to the autobiographical memory model of Conway and collaborators based on the SMS) to more detailed, specific information lower in the hierarchy reinforce depressive rumination (van Vreeswijk & de Wilde,
2004). It has also been found that such difficulties can impair interpersonal problem solving (Williams,
2006) and the ability to imagine the future (e.g., Dickson & Bates,
2006), and that they are also linked to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Williams et al.,
2007).
Chiesa et al. (
2011) reviewed the studies investigating the effects of mindfulness training on AM specificity: they concluded that OGM could be reduced with MBCT. In this section, the review was extended, adding studies published until December 28, 2019, and by including those investigating both the effects of elements of MBCT and those addressing dispositional mindfulness.
This section reviews findings from three cross-sectional, two controlled, and six randomized controlled studies exploring the longitudinal impact of MBCT and of its components on the recall of specific autobiographical memories. The studied populations include formerly depressed patients (Brennan et al.,
2015; Jermann et al.,
2013; Williams et al.,
2000), currently depressed patients (Watkins & Teasdale,
2001,
2004), depressed participants with a history of suicidality (Crane et al.,
2012; Hargus et al.,
2010), healthy adults (Crawley,
2015; Heeren et al.,
2009), and healthy adolescents aged 13–14 (Rice et al.,
2015).
Williams et al. (
2000) found that formerly depressed patients who were in remission showed a significant reduction in overgeneral memories and an improvement in memory specificity after undergoing the MBCT training, compared with a standard psychological relapse prevention treatment (“treatment as usual” group, TAU). These benefits were not attributed to changes in mood scores. Therefore, they were initially explained in terms of a reduction in functional avoidance, with participants being more aware of the retrieval processes. It was also hypothesized that by training participants to focus on specific aspects of their environment and to accept all experiences without judging them, a more specific encoding of events besides a more specific retrieval of past events is reinforced.
Heeren et al. (
2009) replicated Williams et al.’s (
2000) findings. MBCT improved AM specificity and reduced OGM in remitted depressed patients, and extended such results to a healthy sample. Moreover, these authors found that participants in the MBCT group showed an increased ability to inhibit cognitive prepotent responses and an improved cognitive flexibility capacity (i.e., the ability to switch between different cognitive sets) as compared to controls. Moreover, changes in cognitive flexibility partially mediated the impact of mindfulness on improvements in autobiographical memory specificity. However, caution should be taken when generalizing this mediational effect on OGM, as cognitive flexibility was assessed by using a verbal fluency task, which is regarded as a broad measure of executive control (e.g., Rosen & Engle,
1997). Furthermore, this study employed a non-randomized design, thus limiting the capacity to make causal inferences.
Additional support for the positive effects of MBCT on reducing OGM irrespective of mood valence comes from a study that examined this intervention in participants with a history of chronic/recurrent depression and suicidal ideation or behavior (Crane et al.,
2012). Their results suggest that MBCT can increase both the specification of an individual’s personal life goals and the perceived likelihood of their achievement. Furthermore, decreased specificity of aversive memories about relapse signatures was found in suicidal depressed patients’ memories following only TAU as compared to MBCT + TAU (Hargus et al.,
2010).
Rumination, that is, the engagement in repetitive, self-focused negative thought, contributes to OGM (Nolen-Hoeksema,
1991). This cognitive process includes two separable components: a self-focus on symptoms and other aspects of experience and an analytical, evaluative thinking (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow,
1993; Roberts et al.,
1998). Watkins and Teasdale (
2001,
2004) showed that even an 8-min induction of a present-moment experiential mode, without analytical thinking, as in a mindfulness accepting-experience, significantly reduces categorical memory recall in currently depressed patients, as compared to a self-focused analytical mode. The authors have thus suggested that analytical thinking in general, rather than self-focus, is related to OGM, and that a mindful-direct experiential awareness training is capable of modifying it.
There is also some evidence that MBCT may not significantly influence AM specificity (Jermann et al.,
2013). The study of Jermann et al. (
2013) longitudinally tested whether MBCT training affects five cognitive functions (i.e., autobiographical memories, shifting abilities, mindful attention, rumination, and dysfunctional attitudes) in patients remitted from depression. After MBCT + TAU, the only change observed was a significant decrease of dysfunctional attitudes as compared to a TAU-only treatment group. This effect was observed up to 9 months post-intervention. Moreover, even if not significant, the MBCT + TAU group showed increased categorical memories and decreased specific memories at time 2 (after 3 months), compared to TAU only.
These studies compared MBCT to a waiting list, which makes it difficult to distinguish the specific effects of mindfulness training from other interventions. Using a non-randomized longitudinal design, Rice et al. (
2015) compared MBCT with cognitive behavioral therapy and behavioral activation with reward processing (TRY, “Thinking about reward in young people”), as classroom-based prevention programs for depression in healthy adolescents. Unlike the study of Heeren et al. (
2009), which also assessed healthy individuals, a lack of a significantly reduced OGM following MBCT was reported. Rice et al. (
2015) discussed this result in light of recent findings suggesting that OGM may act as a risk factor for depression only in certain high-risk groups of youth (Crane et al.,
2016). Thus, they concluded that mindfulness meditation might not be beneficial for all categories of participants (Farias et al.,
2016).
However, differences in outcomes could also be explained by differences in the tasks used to assess autobiographical memory specificity. Rice et al. (
2015) assessed OGM with the “sentence completion for events from the past test” (SCEPT; Raes et al.,
2007), while all other studies used the classic memory specificity’s assessment paradigm, which is the Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT; Williams & Broadbent,
1986). The main difference between these two tasks is that AMT participants are prompted to search for a specific memory, with the implication of a direct bottom-up retrieval that may lead them to come up with a specific memory, while in the “sentence completion for events from the past test,” they are not, with the involvement of a generative-top-down retrieval. More specifically, in the SCEPT, participants complete sentence stems (e.g., “When I think back to/of…”) with personal memories without being prompted for specificity (i.e., without being explicitly asked to recall specific memories while repeatedly instructed not to respond with overgeneral ones; Raes et al.,
2007). In the AMT, participants read a series of emotionally valenced cue words (e.g., sad, happy), and have to produce a specific memory within a time limit of either 30 or 60 s depending on the study. As already hinted at above, a specific memory refers to a personal past experience that did not last longer than a single day (e.g., “I felt very happy at my wedding last year”).
The four studies reviewed above thus argue that training in state mindfulness affects specificity during autobiographical memory retrieval (i.e., memories have been encoded before the period of training). Moreover, is it possible that individual differences in the ability to be mindful (i.e., trait mindfulness) influence autobiographical memory without mindfulness training and during encoding? Three recent studies investigated the association of trait mindfulness and OGM, and are thus reviewed below. Individual differences in trait mindfulness were firstly found to be associated with memory specificity in a non-clinical student sample (Crawley,
2015). These associations were opposite to those reported in mindfulness training studies: higher trait mindfulness was associated with lower memory specificity, and with more intense and more positive emotion during recall.
Unlike Crawley, Jermann et al. (
2013) found that clinically depressed patients with lower scores of mindful attention/awareness in daily life had less specific memories and impaired executive capacity of shifting, as compared to recurrent depression patients and healthy controls (Jermann et al.,
2013). Accordingly, Brennan et al. (
2015) found no significant correlation between dispositional mindfulness and specific memories in the Autobiographical Memory Test in a clinical sample of individuals with a history of recurrent depression. Derailed reflective thinking was nevertheless associated with more depressive symptoms only in those participants who showed impaired AM specificity and described themselves as less mindful.
Finally, it has to be noted that the studies above measured dispositional levels of mindfulness through different questionnaires which are based on different definitions of mindfulness and related assumptions: the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (Walach et al.,
2006) was used in Crawley’s study; the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan,
2003) in Jermann et al
.’s investigation; and the Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al.,
2006) in Brennan et al.’s work. The interpretation of the discrepancies presented above is made difficult by this lack of conceptual consistency.
The studies providing evidence about the relationship between mindfulness and autobiographical memory specificity are summarized in Table
1.
Table 1
Studies providing evidence about the relationship between mindfulness an autobiographical memory specificity
| RCT | Formerly depressed | 41 | MBCT + TAU/TAU | Standard AMT | Less categoric and more specific memories recalled in the MBCT + TAU group, despite no change in mood scores |
Watkins and Teasdale ( 2001) | RCT | Currently depressed | 36 | Analytical high vs low self-focus/experiential high vs low self-focus | Standard AMT | Decreased proportion of categoric memories in the low analytical (experiential) self-focus condition |
Watkins and Teasdale ( 2004) | RCT | Currently depressed | 28 | Analytical self-focus/experiential self-focus | Standard AMT | Decreased proportion of categoric memories, independently of mood, in the experiential self-focus condition |
| CT | Healthy | 36 | MBCT/no treatment | Standard AMT | Increased number of specific, as well as less categorical and less extended memories after intervention in the MBCT group, partially mediated by cognitive flexibility |
| RCT | Suicidal major depression | 27 | MBCT/waiting list | Relapse signature specificity measure | Less specific recollection of the relapse signature in the waiting list group, but not in the MBCT group; increased meta-awareness in the MBCT group |
| Correlational study | Healthy | 70 | Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory | AMT—minimal instruction version | Higher trait mindfulness associated with lower memory specificity, and with more intense and more positive emotion during recall |
| RCT | Suicidal major depression | 27 | MBCT/waiting list | Standard AMT | Increased goal specificity, associated with increases in autobiographical memory specificity, and likelihood to achieve them, associated with reductions in depressed mood, after MBCT treatment |
| RCT | Formerly depressed | 36 | MBCT + TAU/TAU | Standard AMT | No significant differences for categoric or specific memories observed |
| Cross-sectional design | Formerly depressed/ | 36 | MAAS | Standard AMT | Higher scores of mindful attention/awareness in daily life and less categoric, but more specific memories recalled, in both remitted patients and controls, than acutely depressed patients |
Acutely depressed/ | 20 |
Never depressed | 20 |
| Correlational study | History of recurrent depression | 274 | FFMQ | Standard AMT | Higher levels of reflective thinking associated with more depressive symptoms, only in those with low scores in both mindfulness and memory specificity |
| CT | Healthy adolescents aged 13–14 | 256 | TRY/CBT/MBCT/Controls | SCEPT | None of the conditions showed an overgeneral autobiographical memory significantly reduction |
Mindfulness and Emotional Autobiographical Recall
Autobiographical memory is affected by emotions and their regulation during encoding and retrieval. The rehearsal of emotional memories often produces an emotional response similar to that experienced at event occurrence; and such effect is so powerful that autobiographical event recall is successfully used to induce mood in laboratory settings (Walker et al.,
2009). Since we often try to regulate our emotions while remembering self-relevant events (Holland and Kensinger,
2010) , and as there is robust evidence suggesting that the practice of mindfulness is associated with healthy emotion regulation (Roemer et al.,
2015), it is important to investigate the role of mindfulness when remembering emotional events.
Throughout this section, based on the review results and for their relevance in the framework of the relationships between mindfulness and autobiographical memory, the relationships between mindfulness and emotional recall experiences will be addressed, in order to understand (a) whether and how mindfulness training affects retrieval of mood-incongruent autobiographical memories; (b) whether and how mindfulness training influences the emotions triggered from remembering one’s own past; (c) the underlying neural mechanisms that may support that influence; and (d) the visual perspective during emotional autobiographical memory recall and mindfulness.
Mood-Incongruent Autobiographical Memory Recall and Mindfulness
Individuals tend to retrieve memories more easily when their mood matches the affective valence of the memory: this is called “mood congruence” (for a review, see Blaney,
1986). A study of Greenberg and Meiran (
2014) investigated whether mindfulness meditation practice affects mood-incongruent memory recall, in order to understand if such practice promotes higher or rather lower emotional engagement. Using the Frequency of Autobiographical Memory task (FAM; Sheppes & Meiran,
2007) and self-reported mood checks, in a cross-sectional study, 11 expert mindfulness meditators and 15 matched participants with no formal meditation experience were asked to watch a happy- or a sad-mood-induction film clip. After the movie clip, participants had to rate their current degree of happiness, sadness, and general mood, and quickly recall as many opposite-mood-specific autobiographical memories as possible (i.e., if they watched a sad-mood-induction movie clip, they had to recall happy memories). Researchers measured the recall time for the first opposite-mood memory (i.e., the “recovery speed”) and the total number of memories.
The expert mindfulness meditators took approximately twice the time as compared to the non-meditators to generate the first opposite-mood memory. The authors concluded that an increased engagement in emotional experiences could characterize mindfulness in AM recall. Despite the slower recall time for the first memory, the meditators were, however, able to generate a number of memories comparable to that of the non-meditators. This finding suggests that mindfulness may be associated with a stronger experience of emotion followed by a rapid recovery from it. Moreover, the study found an inconsistency between implicit and explicit emotional experiences in the meditators as compared to the non-meditators, with the Frequency of Autobiographical Memory task’s results more strongly affected by mood inductions than self-reported moods. The latter finding was explained as the result of an enhanced decentering from emotional experiences and an enhanced metacognitive awareness of these experiences in meditators.
Brief Mindfulness Sessions and Self-Relevant Emotional Autobiographical Memories
As noted in the introduction of this section, recalling an emotional autobiographical event often causes a powerful emotional response and is thus used to induce mood in laboratory settings. In order to understand how best to cope with these emotions triggered by self-relevant autobiographical memories, researchers have used brief mindfulness meditation sessions or inductions and compared them to other emotion regulation strategies (such as reappraisal, rumination, and distraction). Emotion regulation is the capacity to influence one person’s emotional experience and/or expression (e.g., Gross,
1998).
Different forms of brief mindfulness practice or induction have been used in these studies, including mindfulness practice elements like focused breathing (Conley et al.,
2018; Fennell et al.,
2016; van den Hout et al.,
2011; Keng & Tan,
2017; Slofstra et al.,
2016), acceptance (Kross et al.,
2009), a combination of focused breathing and acceptance (Broderick,
2005; Cassin & Rector,
2011; Huffziger & Kuehner,
2009; Keng et al.,
2013,
2016a,
b,
2017; Kuehner et al.,
2009; Remmers et al.,
2015,
2016; Singer & Dobson,
2007,
2009), self-focus on emotions and bodily sensations without judging (Ramos Díaz et al.,
2014; Sanders & Lam,
2010; Sauer & Baer,
2012), open monitoring meditation (Conley et al.,
2018), and loving-kindness meditation (Keng & Tan,
2017). The way participants have been instructed to use mindfulness also varies across studies: listening to a mindfulness audiotape (Broderick,
2005; Keng & Tan,
2017; Keng et al.,
2016b); throughout prompts printed on cards (Remmers et al.,
2015,
2016); watched on a personal computer (Sauer & Baer,
2012); and verbally read on (Cassin & Rector,
2011; Keng et al.,
2016b; Singer & Dobson,
2007).
The procedure to induce mood states in a controlled way in most of these investigations usually includes an autobiographical memory recall task: participants are asked to close their eyes and to recall, in progressively increased detail, three specific negative mood-evoking events during which they had felt lonely, sad, embarrassed, rejected, or hurt (“remember everything that happened and how you felt”; Martin,
1990). Some studies differ from this original technique (e.g., van den Fennell et al.,
2016; Hout et al.,
2011), although they have all in common the requirement of remembering and thinking about past personal events, quality that matched inclusion criteria for the present review. For example, Broderick (
2005) used a modified typed-statement Velten’s (
1968) induction procedure that included thinking “about things that happened in their lives that made them feel depressed” (Broderick, 20,005). Following the autobiographical recall phase, participants are asked to apply the emotion regulation strategies (including mindfulness) they had learned before (e.g., van den Hout et al.,
2011; Singer & Dobson,
2007) or after (e.g., Broderick,
2005; Keng et al.,
2016b; Remmers et al.,
2015,
2016) the recalled memory.
Mindful acceptance has been found as effective as distraction in regulating negative mood after remembering negative autobiographical memories, as compared to no training and rumination among previously depressed (Singer & Dobson,
2007), and in currently depressed individuals (Huffziger & Kuehner,
2009). Singer and Dobson also observed an advantage of mindfulness over distraction, with the former resulting in more significant decreases in negative attitudes toward negative memories (i.e., in a belief in the harmfulness of rethinking about negative experiences, resulting in attempts to avoid negative thoughts and feelings associated to them), suggesting that even a brief experiential training in mindful acceptance may enhance a metacognitive stance of awareness and decentering from emotional memories, which allows an adaptive (i.e., not ruminative) exposure to and tolerance of them. Another study by Keng et al. (
2016a) replicated this meta-awareness benefit in comparison with reappraisal among individuals with elevated depressive symptoms, and extended it to positive beliefs (i.e., considering repeated thinking or ruminating about negative experiences as a useful coping strategy), relative to no-training controls. This review section also highlights that dispositional mindfulness may “predispose” individuals for effective mindfulness-based interventions (Huffziger & Kuehner,
2009; Keng et al.,
2013,
2016b; Singer & Dobson,
2009).
More recently, mindfulness has also been compared with reappraisal in individuals with elevated depressive symptoms: both strategies resulted in equivalent decreases in sad moods induced through negative autobiographical recall, although mindfulness was found to be associated with lower cognitive costs as measured by the Stroop task (Keng et al.,
2013). Contrary to expectation, researchers also observed that higher trait mindfulness, but not habitual reappraisal, predicted higher reductions in sadness across conditions.
Brief mindfulness sessions seem to implicate an adaptive emotion-regulation strategy for other negative emotions beyond sadness triggered by autobiographical remembering (e.g., fear, shame, and anger) also for individuals with psychological disorders, such as high borderline personality traits (Keng & Tan,
2017), borderline personality disorder (Sauer & Baer,
2012), and generalized social phobia (Cassin & Rector,
2011). It should be noted, however, that the mood induction used in these studies (i.e., participants were asked to tell about or write the negative autobiographical memory) varies from the one used in previous studies, making it difficult to compare them. Specifically, Keng and Tan (
2017) found that listening to a 10-min mindfulness of breathing exercise audio recording, after writing an upsetting secret incident, helped in the recovery from shame quicker as compared to both another variant of meditation practice (e.g., loving-kindness meditation) and no-intervention. To encourage a mindful observation and awareness of ongoing experience seems to increase distress tolerance, after writing for 10 min about a personal event that had made adults with borderline personality disorder feel angry, significantly longer than training to engage in a ruminative self-focus mode (Sauer & Baer,
2012). Cassin and Rector (
2011) found that mindful acceptance significantly prevented distress when reflecting about an anxious, uncomfortable social or performance experience in generalized social phobia individuals, compared to distraction and no-training controls.
The results in the literature about healthy adults are mixed: brief mindfulness training did not show any effect among undergraduates on a study of Kuehner et al. (
2009), neither among individuals who identified themselves with an Asian self-interdependent context (Keng et al.,
2017). These results are in contrast with a prior study (Broderick,
2005), in which mindful acceptance was shown to be more effective than rumination and distraction in reducing subjective dysphoric mood states in healthy university students.
Further evidence from healthy adults comes from two recent studies of Remmers et al., (
2015,
2016). In both these studies, mindfulness and distraction had a clear positive effect on mood, but only participants allocated to the mindfulness group showed higher congruence between implicit and explicit negative mood measures (Remmers et al.,
2016). The authors suggested that mindfulness may downregulate negative emotion in a more complex and dynamic way than distraction by bringing implicit emotional responses into awareness, i.e., with an explicit conscious access to them; this would lead to an increased congruence of the explicit reports with implicit mood measures.
Using the standard autobiographical mood induction, Conley et al. (
2018) compared the effect of two different mindfulness meditation practices (i.e., focused attention and open monitoring meditation) in healthy undergraduates. They found that, even if both practices helped to reduce distress, the focused attention meditation practice was more beneficial for individuals who reported higher levels of brooding.
Additional support for the effectiveness of brief instructions of mindfulness components on coping with negative memories comes from a study that assessed self-reported and physiological markers of anger (Fennell et al.,
2016). Using a single-subject experimental design, novice and experienced meditators were asked to “really relive” and type a narrative (before and after meditation) about memories of frustration, threats to one’s authority or reputation, disrespect, and/or a sense of injustice or violation of norms or rules. Respiration rate, heart rate, and blood pressure were measured. Experienced meditators did not show any initial indication of sympathetic anger arousal beyond subjective reports, with a limited change of this state after a meditation session. Naive meditators exhibited physiological reactions consistent with anger during the pre-meditation stage; however, after meditation and a second anger induction, they showed physiological evidence of relaxation. Moreover, most of the physiological measures in the novice meditators decreased in a pattern similar to that in routine practitioners. The results revealed that the naive group’s physiological measures mimicked those of the experienced group following a single session of meditation training.
Ramos Díaz et al. (
2014) found that applying mindfulness immediately after the recollection of a stressful event resulted in a better affect balance in female college students, but did not prevent intrusive thoughts about that memory 24 h later. Dispositional mindfulness showed the opposite pattern. These results suggest that the benefits of brief mindfulness inductions have a short-term effect in time, and that a more prolonged and intensive training is needed to cope effectively when remembering acute stressors. Thus, even individuals who usually apply mindfulness in their daily lives may at first find it difficult to cope with memories of highly stressful events if they do not undergo a specific training. Negative autobiographical recall was also used by van den Hout et al. (
2011) under a different experimental procedure. During the first session, participants were required to engage in a mindful attentional breathing practice undertaken while performing a working memory task. During a second session, they were asked to think about previously recalled autobiographical memories that still had a negative emotional impact on them, while mindfully attending to their own breath. They found that mindful breathing reduced vividness (experiment 2) and emotionality (experiment 1) of the memories, besides the increase in reaction time in the working memory task. The authors concluded that MBCT might partly derive its beneficial effects through the capacity of taxing the central executive of working memory with attention directed to the breath during negative memory recall.
Attentional breathing while recalling an aversive autobiographical memory was used as an active control in a recent study that had the aim to test whether perceptual-imagery rescripting (i.e., a mental imagery technique that consists in transforming an aversive memory into a more positive image by changing the meaning-relevant content of it) can reduce unpleasantness, vividness, and emotionality, as compared to conceptual-imagery rescripting and recall only (Slofstra et al.,
2016). Forty-eight healthy students were selected according to their anxious or sad autobiographical memories (one for each condition). They were required to “keep the memory in mind” while applying different instructions (i.e., to change the positions of objects in the perceptual-imagery rescripting instructions; to think about support or help in the conceptual-imagery rescripting instructions; to focus on the breathing during recall with the attentional breathing instructions). Outcome measures were performed before and after each experimental manipulation. It was found that thinking about an aversive memory while mindfully attending breathing was as much effective as changing the perceptual features of the memory on lowering emotionality as compared to the recall-only and conceptual-imagery rescripting conditions. No significant differences were found for the other dependent variables.
The Neural Basis of Mindfulness When Coping with Recollections of Highly Arousing Negative Autobiographical Memories
The section above has highlighted how recalling a negative emotional autobiographical event often causes a powerful emotional response, and how even a brief training in elements of mindfulness often helps to adaptively cope with these emotions. Affective neuroscience studies can lead to an increased understanding of the involved brain regions, networks and processes, and the implicated mechanisms.
To date, only one study conducted by Kross et al. (
2009) provided preliminary functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) evidence in this area. Researchers trained healthy adults to engage in different thought-processing strategies (“feel” emotions naturally flowed through the mind; objectively “analyze” their reasons, similar to reappraisal; and “accept”), as they focused on negative autobiographical memories triggered by cue phrases. During the task, individuals were scanned by fMRI.
Consistent with prior research using image-based stimuli (see Ochsner & Gross,
2008, for a review), all strategies engaged left-lateralized prefrontal areas associated with attention control and reappraisal. Acceptance (“mindfully recognize that the feelings experienced during AM recollection are uncontrolled passing mental events psychologically distant from the self”) was found to be associated with decreased activity in a network of regions involved in self-referential processing, AM recall, and emotion—including the supragenual anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex (Kross et al.,
2009). This pattern of deactivation of brain areas involved in self-related, emotional, and AM processing mirrored participants’ self-reports of lower negative affect in AM recall. Moreover, neural activity patterns in these regions correlated positively with an increase in negative affect on feel versus accept trials, thus indicating that such patterns were directly related to participants’ subjective emotional responses during AM recall.
Visual Perspective During Emotional Autobiographical Memory Recall and Mindfulness
Autobiographical memories can be recalled from two different perspectives: the first-person “field” perspective, in which the scene of the memory is reconstructed from the individual’s original point of view, and the third-person “observer” perspective, in which the rememberers “see” themselves as actors in the memory image (Freud,
1899; Nigro & Neisser,
1983; Robinson & Swanson,
1993).
Kuyken and Moulds (
2009) explored the vividness and vantage perspective of memories recalled with the AMT, which would be relevant from the perspective of decentering in meditation, and showed that patients with a history of recurrent depression tend to retrieve observer-perspective memories. Furthermore, this was associated with lower dispositional mindfulness, higher negative self-evaluation, and higher use of avoidance. In line with Lemogne et al. (
2006) and Williams and Moulds (
2007), the authors argued that depressive patients might use the observer-perspective vantage as a form of cognitive avoidance. Although Kuyken and Moulds’ results suggest that decentering from one’s own memories (i.e., by adopting a third-person perspective), combined with negative self-evaluation, is particularly maladaptive in the context of clinical depression, other clinical researchers consider decentering as a central mechanism in mindfulness-based treatment for depressed populations (see Bernstein et al.,
2015). In comparing the different studies, it seems important to highlight that the memories retrieved in Kuyken and Moulds’ study were not spontaneous intrusive memories, but rather memories which were intentionally retrieved and elicited in response to cue words, which might have influenced the vantage perspective.
The studies providing evidence for mindfulness as an emotional self-regulation strategy following autobiographical remembering are summarized in Table
2.
Table 2
Studies providing evidence for mindfulness as an emotional self- regulation strategy following autobiographical remembering
| RCT | Healthy undergraduate | 177 | Mindful acceptance/rumination/distraction/no training | Negative autobiographical recall | Significantly lower levels of negative mood in the mindfulness group |
| RCT | Remitted depressed | 80 | Mindful acceptance/rumination/distraction/no training | Negative autobiographical recall | Decreased intensity of negative mood in both distraction and acceptance groups; greater reduction in negative attitudes toward negative experience only in the acceptance group |
| CT | Remitted depressed | 65 | Acceptance engaged/non-acceptance | Negative autobiographical recall | Greater negative mood reduction and increased metacognitive beliefs about acceptance after the use of the technique in participants who optimally engaged in acceptance compared to those who failed to adhere to it; higher levels of anxiety and greater negative attitudes toward sadness negatively predicted the ability to engage in acceptance |
| RCT | Healthy undergraduate | 60 | Mindful self-focus/rumination/distraction | Negative autobiographical recall | No significant effect on reducing dysphoric mood of mindful self-focus compared to rumination; increased dysfunctional attitudes only in the rumination group; no significant decreases in dysfunctional attitudes in distraction and mindful self-focus groups from baseline |
Huffziger and Kuehner ( 2009) | RCT | Currently depressed | 76 | Mindful self-focus/rumination/distraction | Negative autobiographical recall | Significant mood improvement in the mindful self-focus and distraction groups compared to rumination; high mindfulness trait enhanced this effect in the mindfulness group |
| RCT Within subjects | Healthy | 16 | Acceptance/feel/analyze strategies | fMRI while focusing on negative autobiographical memories | Lower activity in sgACC and mPFC and lower self-reported negative affect for the accept strategy vs the feel strategy |
| Cross-sectional | Depressed | 123 | KIMS | AMT | Mindfulness positively correlated with both positive and negative field memories recalled |
| CT | Dysphoric with/without history of depression | 43 | Analytical self-focus/ Experiential self-focus | Negative autobiographical recall | No significant differences in SPS between groups after training; scoring high on trait rumination predicted significant improvements in SPS after induced mindful-experiential |
| RCT | Generalized social phobia | 57 | Mindful acceptance/distraction/no training | Negative autobiographical recall | Significantly more positive affect over the post-event period only in the mindfulness group |
van den Hout et al. ( 2011) | Within subjects | Healthy | 72 | Eye movements/attentional breathing/recall only | Negative autobiographical recall | Attentional breathing reduced emotionality but not vividness compared to recall only in experiment 1; attentional breathing reduced vividness but not emotionality in experiment 2 |
| RCT | Borderline personality disorder | 40 | Mindful self-focus/ruminative self-focus | Angry autobiographical recall | Significant longer distress tolerance in the mindfulness group than in the ruminative group |
| RCT | Elevated depressive symptoms | 100 | Mindful acceptance/reappraisal/no training | Negative autobiographical recall | Faster recovery from sad mood in the mindfulness and reappraisal groups compared to no training; less cognitive costs in the mindfulness group; higher trait mindfulness predicted greater decreases in sadness across conditions |
Greenberg and Meiran ( 2014) | CT | Healthy | 11 13 | Vipassana meditators/ No practice | FAM | Increased emotional engagement; increased contact with emotions, and rapid recovery from the emotional experience in the meditators group |
| RCT | Healthy female | 76 | Mindfulness/analytical/no instruction | Recollection of an acute stressor | Increased positive affect and decreased negative affect after being instructed to apply mindfulness compared to no instruction; fewer intrusive thoughts associated with high trait mindfulness |
| RCT | Healthy | 75 | Mindfulness/rumination/distraction | Negative autobiographical recall | Scoring high on “acting without judgment” and trait general mindfulness negatively associated with access to intuitive processes; no significant differences across conditions on intuition; improved negative affect in mindfulness and distraction groups |
| RCT | Healthy | 72 | Mindfulness/rumination/distraction | Negative autobiographical recall | Significant decrease of implicit and explicit negative mood in the mindfulness and distraction conditions |
| RCT | Elevated depressive symptoms | 100 | Mindful acceptance/reappraisal/no training | Negative autobiographical recall | Greater decreases in maladaptive (positive and negative) beliefs about rumination in the mindfulness acceptance group |
| Within subjects | Healthy | 48 | Attentional breathing/conceptual-imagery rescripting/perceptual imagery rescripting/recall only | Negative autobiographical recall | Decreased emotionality in the recall + attentional breathing condition compared to the recall-only and conceptual-imagery rescripting condition |
| CT | Healthy | 15 12 | Novice meditators Practiced meditators | Angry autobiographical recall | No initial indication of sympathetic anger arousal beyond subjective report and limited change following meditation in the practiced group; physiological reactions consistent with anger during the pre-meditation stage, but physiological evidence of relaxation after meditation and a second anger induction in the practiced group |
| RCT | Elevated depressive symptoms | 59 | Mindful acceptance/thought wandering | Negative autobiographical recall | Improved implicit dysfunctional attitudes associated with high trait mindfulness in the mindful acceptance group; greater levels of concordance between implicit and explicit dysfunctional attitudes in the mindful acceptance group |
| RCT | Healthy Asian undergraduate | 125 | Mindfulness/reappraisal/suppression | Negative autobiographical recall | Faster sadness recovery in mindfulness compared to suppression; lower average sadness of suppression compared to mindfulness associated with high, but not low, Asian cultural values; lower cognitive costs of mindfulness and reappraisal compared to suppression |
| RCT | High borderline personality traits | 71 | MB/LKM/no instruction | Shame autobiographical recall | Faster shame recovery in the MB group compared to the LKM group |
| RCT | Healthy | 228 | FA/OM | Negative autobiographical recall | Significant reduction of distress in both conditions; high brooding levels moderated focused attention effect on decreased distress |
Self-Inquiry into Negative Autobiographical Narratives and Mindfulness
As considered above, brief mindfulness training can enhance our ability to cope with emotions triggered from remembering negative autobiographical experiences. When life events maintained in autobiographical memory are represented in a stabilized and coherent self-related form, in a way that implies the construction of a story that can be shared with other individuals, they become part of our personal narrative (Smorti,
2011).
A task that is explicitly linked to retrieval of highly negative autobiographical narratives is the “narrative, emotional disclosure task” (Pennebaker & Beall,
1986). It is an expressive writing paradigm, in which participants are required to write repeated narratives about upsetting or highly negative experiences (e.g., 15–20 min per day for 3–5 days). This task has been shown to lead to positive outcomes, such as healthier immune functioning (see Frattaroli,
2006, for a meta-analysis).
It has been proposed that the self-inquiry involved in autobiographical narrative writing may enhance awareness of internal states, through a free-associative unrestrictive expression, in a way comparable to mindfulness (Brody & Park,
2004). Since emotions and their regulation during memory recollection often are not the same as the ones experienced at the time of the event, one possible explanation of the abovementioned benefits could be that while engaging in the task, participants may relive the experiences as if they were happening in the here and now, allowing memories for emotions to be reconstructed—updated—based on new information in the perspective of the present (Levine & Safer,
2002).
The next section will review the role that mindfulness plays when self-inquiring upsetting or traumatic memories through autobiographical narrative writing. Specifically, it will cover (a) trait mindfulness and disclosure of traumatic autobiographical narratives and (b) the influences of mindfulness and self-compassion on emotions triggered from disclosure of upsetting or traumatic autobiographical narratives.
Trait Mindfulness and Disclosure of Traumatic Autobiographical Narratives
A particular kind of negative autobiographical narrative is the narrative of traumatic experiences, as it is often described as vivid representations involving sensory impressions and a sense of reliving, and is associated with the development and maintenance of posttraumatic stress reactions (Brewin et al.,
1996; Ehlers & Clark,
2000; Rubin et al.,
2008). Three randomized studies have examined the extent to which dispositional mindfulness might moderate self-inquiry during the disclosure of traumatic experiences through expressive writing.
In a first study, repeated writing of traumatic narratives resulted in linguistic changes (i.e., more present tense and cognitive processing words), in association with improvements in mindfulness skills (Moore & Brody,
2009). Cross-sectional analyses of baseline mindfulness and linguistic categories on the first day of writing indicated that when participants used less self-referenced words, they scored higher in observing internal and external stimuli, as assessed by a mindfulness subscale of the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS; Baer et al.,
2004). Authors argued that their finding is consistent with the Buddhist view of mindfulness as an experience of “selflessness” (Harvey,
1995; Williams,
2008), lessening the importance of individual self-identity (see also above in the introduction).
Simply writing about traumatic experiences may not be sufficient to reduce the levels of psychological distress associated with remembering a traumatic event. Only college students that showed an increase in mindfulness from baseline to follow-up showed less psychological distress at a 1-month follow-up after Pennebaker and Beall’s task (Moore et al.,
2009). These findings suggest that the presumed benefits of the narrative emotional disclosure task are indeed related to increases in mindfulness skills.
Poon and Danoff-Burg (
2011) have also shown that, in participants who scored higher in mindfulness at baseline, expressive writing induced higher reductions in physical and psychological symptoms and negative affect, and led to increased sleep quality and positive affect. Thus, it seems that only individuals able to be mindfully aware of their thoughts and emotions while disclosing their traumatic experiences benefit from written expression. Poon and Danoff-Burg (
2011) suggest that enhanced mindfulness-type attention to the writing process may also partially explain their results.
The Influences of Mindfulness and Self-Compassion on Emotions Triggered from Disclosure of Upsetting or Traumatic Autobiographical Narratives
The way in which mindfulness exerts its beneficial effects when coping with highly arousing emotions triggered from disclosure of upsetting or traumatic narratives has been a topic of much research.
The majority of these studies have used writing prompts to induce self-compassion. Self-compassion can be defined as a way of relating to oneself, which has three core components, including the mindfulness component (i.e., mindful awareness of our own negative thoughts and emotions so that they can be approached with balance and equanimity) (Neff & Dahm,
2015). The other two components of self-compassion are the recognition that other people also experience similar events (i.e., common humanity) and a caring perspective toward oneself through understanding and self-acceptance (i.e., self-kindness).
Leary et al. (
2007) developed and used the self-compassion writing task. This task required undergraduates to write about a negative event from a self-compassionate perspective, induced by prompts. They found that self-compassion promotes significantly lower levels of post-writing negative affect, as compared to writing that boosts self-esteem, or in a traditional way (Leary et al.,
2007), even when participants indicated that the negative event was more likely to be caused by their characteristics. Researchers inferred that these results might be due to differences in the degree to which they encouraged participants to focus on themselves: self-esteem kept participants focused on themselves instead of widening their perspective to include other people, who instead took place in self-compassion.
Self-compassionate writing benefits were replicated by Johnson and O’Brien (
2013) with shame-eliciting memories through repeated expressive writing (3 times a week), by Odou and Brinker (
2015) compared to distraction and after a negative mood induction (Odou & Brinker,
2014), and recently by DeLury and Poulin (
2018), with the disclosure of a negative academic event experienced in high school or college, involving failure, humiliation, or rejection. Specifically, in this last study, self-compassion buffered participants’ academic task performance.
Only one study reported negative outcomes (i.e., significantly higher levels of negative affect) associated with self-compassion expressive writing among Chinese individuals (Wong & Mak,
2016). However, a 1-month and a 3-month follow-up showed health benefits (i.e., a significant reduction in physical symptoms) associated with self-compassion that were not observed in the traditional control writing group.
Some preliminary evidence has also shown that traditional expressive writing prevents depressive symptoms when augmented by emotion-acceptance instructions (Baum & Rude,
2013), and decreases event centrality when augmented by acceptance and decentering (Boals et al.,
2015). Event centrality is the extent to which a traumatic event is perceived as central to one's identity and sense of self (Berntsen & Rubin,
2006).
Finally, the narrative emotional disclosure task has also been used as a dependent variable for measuring changes in emotions and mindfulness related to adverse childhood attachment experiences. Caldwell and Shaver (
2015) used a 3-day mindfulness-based intervention, which was specifically designed to apply mindfulness and loving-kindness techniques in female participants with a history of traumatic attachment experiences. Post-intervention results showed increases in language-based mindfulness compared to a waiting list control group: fewer past tense, more present tense, cognitive processing, and insight-oriented words, and this was associated with less suppression and better emotion regulation over time.
The processing of a traumatic memory has been investigated using other tasks other than the one from Pennebaker and Beall. For example, Shors et al. (
2018) used questions from the Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (AMQ; Rubin et al.,
2003), in order to measure the strength of the most stressful life memory in a group of women with and without a history of sexual violence. Participants were then allocated by randomization to training sessions (two sessions a week for at least 6 weeks) that included meditation alone, aerobic exercises alone, and a combination of them, or to a control group with no training. Only in the group with no training was an increased memory strength (i.e., reliving, seeing, and hearing the event) found, thus suggesting a preventative effect of training regardless of the meditation component.
Valdez and Lilly adapted the catastrophizing interview (Davey & Levy,
1998; Vasey & Borkovec,
1992) to assess perseverative thinking about the trauma index of women that had been victims of interpersonal trauma. Using this trauma-specific perseverative thinking interview, after participants randomly assigned into three processing conditions underwent an induction (i.e., “analytic,” or with brooding and conceptualizing; “experiential,” or self-reflective with mindful experiencing; and control), they observed that the processing modes exerted differential effects of self-compassion on trauma-related anxiety (Valdez & Lilly,
2016). Interestingly, self-kindness and common humanity correlated with more anxiety after the experiential induction, although positive affect was found to be increased, whereas positive outcomes were observed after the analytical induction and in the control condition. In agreement with King et al. (
2013), the authors concluded that mindfulness exercises that involve attending to bodily states might increase anxiety by triggering traumatic memories of an assault. Valdez and Lilly (
2019) further observed that higher scores in self-kindness and mindfulness at baseline were associated with a diminished increase in guilt after trauma processing. This protective effect was only significant in the analytic group, which suggests that self-compassion may have a stronger effect on affective guilt when people focus on the causes and consequences of traumatic events.
The studies providing evidence for the relationship between self-inquiry into negative autobiographical narratives and mindfulness are summarized in Table
3.
Table 3
Studies providing evidence about the relationship between self-inquiry into negative autobiographical narratives and mindfulness
| RCT | Healthy | 115 | Self-compassion/Self-esteem/Expressive writing-only control/True control | Narrative disclosure task | Lower negative affect and higher responsibility in the self-compassion group |
| RCT | Healthy | 233 | KIMS | Narrative disclosure task | Increased non-judgmental acceptance after writing about daily lives; higher baseline mindfulness and increases in mindfulness from baseline to follow-up, associated with mental health in the experimental-emotional group |
| RCT | Healthy | 233 | KIMS | Narrative disclosure task | Linguistic changes—cognitive processing and present tense words—in self-disclosure narratives, significantly predicted increases in mindfulness |
Poon and Danoff-Burg ( 2011) | RCT | Healthy | 76 | FMI | Shame narrative disclosure task | Higher in mindfulness at baseline, evidenced greater reductions in physical and psychological symptoms, negative affect, and increased sleep quality and positive affect |
| RCT | Healthy | 90 | Self-compassion/expressive writing/no-writing control | Narrative disclosure task | Lower levels of self-reported shame-proneness, rumination and negative affect only in the self-compassion writing group |
| RCT | High vs low depression symptoms | 218 | Acceptance expressive writing/expressive writing/writing control | Narrative disclosure task | Higher forestall symptoms of depression at a 5-week follow-up, for mild to severe depression baseline, in the acceptance expressive writing group |
| RCT | Healthy | 186 | Self-compassion expressive writing/expressive writing | Narrative disclosure task | Improved mood at T2 in the S-C group; trait rumination inhibited, while S-C predicted mood improvements across conditions |
| RCT | Healthy | 152 | Self-compassion expressive writing/distraction task | Narrative disclosure task | Increased positive affect at T2 in the S-C; high rumination predicted greater reduction of sadness |
Caldwell and Shaver ( 2015) | RCT | Female with a history of traumatic attachment experiences | 48 | Loving-kindness-mindfulness writing/waiting list | Narrative disclosure task | Increased language-based mindfulness in the treatment group: fewer past tense, more present tense, cognitive processing, and insight-oriented words; less suppression and better emotion regulation over time |
| RCT | Healthy with high PTSD scores | 79 | ACT/CBT/control | Narrative disclosure task | Significant decreases in the event centrality in the ACT and CBT groups |
| RCT | Healthy Chinese | 65 | Self-compassion expressive writing/writing control | Narrative disclosure task | Higher negative affect immediately after self-compassion expressive writing, but significant reduction in physical symptoms at 1-month and 3-month follow-up |
| RCT | Women with/without a history of sexual violence | 105 | FA meditation/aerobic exercises/FA meditation + aerobic exercises/no training | AMQ | No changes in memory’s strength for any of the training groups; significantly increased memory’s strength only in the no-training group |
| RCT | Interpersonal trauma women | 63 | Analytic/experiential/control | Trauma-specific perseverative thinking interview | Higher baseline self-kindness and mindfulness associated with a diminished increase in guilt after trauma processing, mainly in the analytic group |
| RCT | Interpersonal trauma women | 63 | Analytic/experiential/control | Trauma-specific perseverative thinking interview | Self-kindness and mindfulness negatively correlated with negative affect at T2 in controls; self-kindness negatively correlated with negative affect; mindfulness negatively correlated with anxiety and negative affect, and positively correlated with positive affect in the analytic group at T2; self-kindness positively correlated with anxiety and positive affect; common humanity positively correlated with anxiety in the experiential group at T2 |
| RCT | Healthy | 333 | Self-compassion writing/expressive writing | Self-esteem threat autobiographical recall/neutral autobiographical recall | Self-compassion prevented impaired academic task performance; marginally impacting implicit non-evaluative self-thoughts, and not affecting evaluative thoughts or implicit self-esteem |
Mindfulness and Flashbacks
In the previous section, we reviewed evidence about the role of mindfulness in recalling autobiographical memories that are deliberately brought to consciousness. However, some autobiographical memories are not consciously recalled. A kind of autobiographical memory, i.e., a flashback, is intrusive; thus, it comes into consciousness though unbidden. Flashbacks force the individual to re-experience traumatic events. During a flashback, a person is completely absorbed in the memory and temporarily loses contact with the here and now (Mace,
2007). Re-experiencing traumatic events (e.g., in the form of intrusive trauma memories, nightmares, and distress in reaction to trauma reminders) is a common symptom for the diagnosis of PTSD.
Since the aim of this article is to provide a comprehensive review of the studies using different paradigms to investigate the effects of mindfulness training on AM, here, we include an additional section that reviews 18 studies investigating the impact of MT on unwanted traumatic memories, or flashbacks
. Given that most measures for the assessment of PTSD address intrusive thoughts and images as a component of re-experiencing memories, in this section, studies that assess flashbacks using self-report measures for traumatic stress are included (e.g., the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale—CAPS; Blake et al.,
1995; the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV -SCID—First et al.,
1995).
Although there is robust cross-sectional support for the influence of mindfulness on the re-experience of trauma symptoms in PTSD (e.g., Seligowski et al.,
2015), some studies did not find significant associations (Karatzias et al.,
2018; Maheux & Price,
2015; Thompson & Waltz,
2008). For example, in order to study flashback’s qualities, Malaktaris and Lynn (
2019) compared individuals with posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS, i.e., with PTSD or subthreshold PTSD symptoms) with and without flashbacks to trauma-exposed controls and control participants without trauma exposure. Dispositional mindfulness (assessed through the MAAS), general mindfulness skills (assessed through the FFMQ), and the mindfulness facets describing, acting with awareness, and non-judgment (also assessed through the FFMQ) were significantly lower in PTSS. However, these measures did not differ between the participants with and without flashbacks. These results suggest that mindfulness traits are related to other PTSD symptoms. Moreover, trait mindfulness did not predict who would develop flashbacks upon similar trauma exposure. Re-experiencing has been found to be negatively associated with the mindfulness facets non-judging (Chopko & Schwartz,
2013; Reffi et al.,
2019; Vujanovic et al.,
2009; Wahbeh et al.,
2011), non-reactivity (Kalill et al.,
2014), and acting with awareness (Gonzalez et al.,
2016; Reffi et al.,
2019; Vujanovic et al.,
2009). However, further research appears needed to understand better how the different aspects of mindfulness may operate in reducing flashbacks, in order to clarify the somewhat discrepant findings.
Flashbacks were investigated through self-reported questionnaires and clinical interviews in twelve uncontrolled and six randomized controlled studies. Almost all studies reported improvements in intrusions and re-experiencing symptoms. In most studies, the participants were veterans (Bremner et al.,
2017; Felleman et al.,
2016; Jasbi et al.,
2018; Kearney et al.,
2012; King et al.,
2013,
2016; Shipherd et al.,
2016; Stephenson et al.,
2017). Participants also included: incarcerated women with histories of childhood sexual and/or physical abuse (Bradley & Follingstad,
2003); refugees (Hinton et al.,
2005); survivors of childhood sexual abuse (Earley et al.,
2014; Kimbrough et al.,
2010); patients treated for cancer (Bränström et al.,
2012); nurses with subclinical PTSD (Kim et al.,
2013); survivors of interpersonal violence (Müller-Engelmann et al.,
2017,
2019); and psychology students that had been exposed to lifetime trauma, or had experienced life stress in the past year (Zhu et al.,
2019).
Affective neuroscience studies can lead to an increased understanding of the brain regions, networks, processes, and thus of the mechanisms implicated in flashbacks. In functional neuroimaging these improvements have been associated with an increased pre- to post-intervention default mode network (posterior cingular cortex seed) connectivity to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regions within the central executive network and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex within the salience network, with an increased activity of anterior cingulate cortex and inferior parietal lobule, and a decreased activity of the insula and precuneus in response to traumatic reminders (Bremner et al.,
2017). King et al. (
2016) also found that functional connectivity between posterior cingulate cortex (default mode network) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (central executive network) was correlated with improvement in PTSD avoidant and hyperarousal symptoms.
One study investigated how a culturally adapted cognitive-behavioral therapy, which includes acceptance and mindfulness techniques (i.e., focused attention upon specific sensory modalities and multisensorial awareness of the present moment), helped Cambodian refugees with treatment-resistant PTSD. Hinton et al. (
2005) used a Flashback Severity Scale (FSS) to assess the severity of the flashbacks associated with panic attacks, and they found that flashbacks significantly improved with treatment. Their results suggest that mindfulness helps individuals to extinguish fear responses to highly traumatic memories successfully, by bringing acceptance to bodily, affective responses, and to refrain from engaging in internal reactivity toward them. A note of caution should be sounded, however, as it is not clear whether it is the mindfulness or the cognitive techniques that cause these effects.
Soldiers who received acceptance-based training demonstrated more pronounced reductions in frequency, distress, and impairment associated with deployment-related intrusive cognitions at 1-month follow-up relative to participants who underwent a change-oriented skills training, which stops and replaces intrusive cognitions with more pleasant experiences (Shipherd et al.,
2016).
There is also initial evidence that mindfulness may not be so helpful when its components are isolated. Valdez et al. (
2016) found that only after being induced to think in a non-judgmental way did women with interpersonal victimization histories write present-focused words that were inversely related to trauma intrusion frequency (Valdez et al.,
2016). This effect was not present in the absence of a non-judgmental induction, which means that the cultivation of a non-judgmental attitude appears to be essential to protect against intrusive memories.
The studies providing evidence about the relationship between mindfulness and flashbacks are summarized in Table
4.
Table 4
Studies providing evidence about the relationship between mindfulness and flashbacks
Bradley and Follingstad ( 2003) | RCT Pre-post-treatment measures | Incarcerated women experiencing interpersonal violence | 31 | DBT No treatment | TSI-Intrusive experiences | Significant intrusive experiences reduction at posttreatment in the DBT group (d = 1.0); increased intrusive experiences in the control group |
| Cross-over, with n = 20 in initial treatment and n = 20 in delayed treatment | Cambodian refugees | 40 | BCT with elements of MF | N-FSS O-FSS | Reductions of flashbacks over time |
| No control groups Pre-treatment measures and 4/8/24 weeks follow-up | CSA survivors | 27 | MBSR | PCL | Re-experiencing reductions at 4 weeks (d = 0.8), at 8 weeks (d = 0.8), and at 24 weeks (d = 0.9) |
| RCT Pre-treatment measures and 6 months follow-up | Cancer diagnosis | 58 | MBSR/waiting list | IES-intrusion | Decreased intrusions on both groups; no significant differences between the intervention group and the control group |
| No control groups Pre-post-measures and 6 months follow-up | Veterans | 92 | MBSR | PCL-C | Re-experiencing reductions at posttreatment (d = 0.40) and at follow-up |
| RCT Pre-post-measures and 8 weeks follow-up | Nurses with PTSD | 22 | MBX/ Control | PCL | Re-experiencing reductions at posttreatment in both groups, but statistically significant only in the MBX group |
| RCT Pre-post-measures | Veterans | 28 | MBCT/ TAU | CAPS-I PDS | Trend to significantly decreased intrusions in the MBCT group; increased intrusions in the TAU group |
| No control groups Pre-treatment measures and 128 weeks follow-up | CSA survivors | 19 | MBSR | PCL | Re-experiencing reductions at posttreatment, and at 24 weeks (d = 0.9) and 128 weeks follow-up (d = 0.5) |
| RCT Pre-post-measures | Veterans | 23 | MBET/ PCGT | CAPS-I | Re-experiencing reductions at posttreatment in both groups, but statistically significant only in the MBET group (d = 0.72); increased activity in DLPFC and ACC regions following MBET; increased connectivity with DLPFC and dorsal ACC following therapy |
| No control groups Pre-post-measures and 4 months follow-up | Veterans | 116 | MBSR | PCL—re-experiencing subscale | Significant re-experiencing symptoms reduction at posttreatment and at follow-up |
| No control groups Pre-post measures | Veterans | 113 | MBSR | PCL- Re-experiencing subscale | Re-experiencing reductions at posttreatment (d = 0.51) |
| No control groups Pre-post measures | Women experiencing interpersonal violence | 40 | Non-judgment Controls | TLP | Present-focused words inversely related to trauma intrusion frequency only in the non-judgment group |
| RCT Pre-post measures | Post-deployment soldiers | 1524 | Psychoeducation + acceptance Psychoeducation + change focused Psychoeducation only TAU | EIS | Significant decrease in EIS scores in the acceptance-based condition, compared to all other conditions, which did not differ with each other |
Müller-Engelmann et al. ( 2017) | No control groups Pre-post-measures and 1 month follow-up | Interpersonal violence | 14 | MBSR | CAPS-I | Re-experiencing reductions at follow-up (d = 1.4) |
| RCT Pre-post-measures | Veterans | 48 | MBCT | PCL | Re-experiencing reductions at posttreatment in both groups, but statistically significant only in the MBCT group (d = 2.06) |
| RCT Pre-post-measures and 6 months follow-up | Veterans | 17 | MBSR/PCGT | CAPS-I | Nonstatistically significant reduction of intrusions in the MBSR group; nonstatistically significant increase of intrusions in the MBSR group; increased activities of ACC and inferior parietal lobule, and decreased activities of the insula and precuneus in response to traumatic reminders in the MBSR group compared to the PCGT group |
Müller-Engelmann et al. ( 2019) | Multiple baseline across individuals Pre-post-measures | Interpersonal violence | 12 | MBSR + loving-kindness meditation | CAPS-I | Re-experiencing reductions at follow-up (d = 0.45) |
| No control groups Pre-post-measures | Psychology students that had been exposed to lifetime trauma, or had experienced life stress in the past year | 151 | 5 min of focused attention mindfulness exercise | ICSRLE ACEs PCL-5 TRASC ITQ D-PTSD SCL-10R | Increased traumatic re-experiencing and intrusive memories during meditation, only in those who had developed posttraumatic symptoms |