Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2014

01-05-2014 | Original Article

Auditory spatial negative priming: What is remembered of irrelevant sounds and their locations?

Auteurs: Susanne Mayr, Malte Möller, Axel Buchner

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The categorization and identification of previously ignored visual or auditory stimuli is typically slowed down—a phenomenon that has been called the negative priming effect and can be explained by the episodic retrieval of response-inadequate prime information and/or an inhibitory model. A similar after-effect has been found in visuospatial tasks: participants are slowed down in localizing a visual stimulus that appears at a previously ignored location. In the auditory modality, however, such an after-effect of ignoring a sound at a specific location has never been reported. Instead, participants are impaired in their localization performance when the sound at the previously ignored location changes identity, a finding which is compatible with the so-called feature-mismatch hypothesis. Here, we describe the properties of auditory spatial in contrast to visuospatial negative priming and report two experiments that specify the nature of this auditory after-effect. Experiment 1 shows that the detection of identity-location mismatches is a genuinely auditory phenomenon that can be replicated even when the sound sources are invisible. Experiment 2 reveals that the detection of sound-identity mismatches in the probe depends on the processing demands in the prime. This finding implies that the localization of irrelevant sound sources is not the inevitable consequence of processing the auditory prime scenario but depends on the difficulty of the target search process among distractor sounds.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Note that direct comparisons between ignored repetition and attended repetition trials are ambiguous because they do not only differ in whether there is a stimulus and/or location repetition of the to-be-ignored or the to-be-attended sound. What is more, attended repetition and ignored repetition trials also differ in whether they contain a response repetition or not. As a consequence, performance differences between ignored and attended repetition trials can be the result of differences in (a) attentional allocation to the identity and/or location repetitions, (b) response repetition, and/or (c) interactions between both variables. Since our focus was on the after-effects of ignoring sounds at spatial positions and given the interpretational problems in comparing attended and ignored repetition trials, we decided to restrict all analyses reported to the ignored repetition design.
 
2
Figure 1 indicates that the marginally non-significant comparison between sound-repeated, location-changed trials and sound-changed, location-changed control trials was due to the visible loudspeakers group whereas the concealed loudspeakers group showed a clear slow-down for sound-repeated, location-changed mismatch trials. Note that we have found evidence of this type of a feature-mismatch in a previous, almost-identical experiment with loudspeaker visibility (Mayr, Buchner, et al., 2011). While we cannot exclude that there might be a substantial explanation for the absence of a slow-down in sound-repeated, location-changed trials in the visible loudspeakers group of the present experiment, we think it best to attribute this datapoint to a type two error.
 
3
Data from an as-yet-unpublished experiment of our group show that the spatial separation between two sound sources improves detection performance of a to-be-searched musical instrument when the musical sounds are of high spectral similarity (same fundamental frequency). When the sounds differed in their spectral properties (different fundamentals; 330 and 466 Hz), the spatial separation of the two sounds did not increase the sensitivity of target detection. In line with the existing literature, stream segregation in this task seems to be solved based on the spectral information when possible. Only when the frequency-based segregation of sound sources turns extremely difficult, spatial cues are taken into account.
 
4
This comparison process is probably most similar to what Bregman (1990) described as a schema-driven process of auditory scene analysis that looks for matching patterns between representations of earlier acoustic experiences and the present sound wave mixture. This mode of analysis stands in contrast to a preattentive process that segments the acoustic input based on its physical similarities and dissimilarities following gestalt principles (see Alain & Arnott, 2000).
 
5
Note that there was a (non-significant) tendency to be slowed down in probe responding for the auditory as compared to the visual prime cue group. Although not statistically significant, such an effect would not have been surprising. Keep in mind that the two prime cue groups did only differ with respect to the modality of the prime cue, not the probe cue. The probe cuing was always visual. This implies that participants in the auditory prime cue group had to switch from auditory prime to visual probe cuing which was certainly more demanding than the task in the visual prime cue group with visual cues in prime and probe.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alain, C., & Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Frontiers in bioscience : A journal and virtual library, 5, 202–212.CrossRef Alain, C., & Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Frontiers in bioscience : A journal and virtual library, 5, 202–212.CrossRef
go back to reference Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sounds. London: The MIT press. Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sounds. London: The MIT press.
go back to reference Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bronkhorst, A. (2000). The cocktail party phenomenon: A review of research on speech intelligibility in multiple-talker conditions. Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 86, 117–128. Bronkhorst, A. (2000). The cocktail party phenomenon: A review of research on speech intelligibility in multiple-talker conditions. Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 86, 117–128.
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Avramidis, C., & Fitzgeorge, L. (2008). Prime-trial processing demands and their impact on distractor processing in a spatial negative priming task. Psychological Research, 72(3), 235–248.PubMedCrossRef Buckolz, E., Avramidis, C., & Fitzgeorge, L. (2008). Prime-trial processing demands and their impact on distractor processing in a spatial negative priming task. Psychological Research, 72(3), 235–248.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Edgar, C., Kajaste, B., Lok, M., & Khan, M. (2012). Inhibited prime-trial distractor responses solely produce the visual spatial negative priming effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(8), 1632–1643. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0366-0.CrossRef Buckolz, E., Edgar, C., Kajaste, B., Lok, M., & Khan, M. (2012). Inhibited prime-trial distractor responses solely produce the visual spatial negative priming effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(8), 1632–1643. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-012-0366-0.CrossRef
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Goldfarb, A., & Khan, M. (2004). The use of a distractor-assigned response slows later responding in a location negative priming task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(5), 837–845. doi:10.3758/BF03194977.CrossRef Buckolz, E., Goldfarb, A., & Khan, M. (2004). The use of a distractor-assigned response slows later responding in a location negative priming task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(5), 837–845. doi:10.​3758/​BF03194977.CrossRef
go back to reference Chao, H.-F. (2009). Revisiting the role of probe distractors in negative priming: Location negative priming is observed when probe distractors are consistently absent. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(5), 1072–1082. doi:10.3758/APP.71.5.1072.CrossRef Chao, H.-F. (2009). Revisiting the role of probe distractors in negative priming: Location negative priming is observed when probe distractors are consistently absent. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(5), 1072–1082. doi:10.​3758/​APP.​71.​5.​1072.CrossRef
go back to reference Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 19, 988–992. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 19, 988–992. doi:10.​1121/​1.​1907229.
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Coles, M. G. H., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(5), 529–553. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.529.PubMed Coles, M. G. H., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(5), 529–553. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​11.​5.​529.PubMed
go back to reference Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1993). Cross-modal negative priming and interference in selective attention. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(1), 45–48.CrossRef Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1993). Cross-modal negative priming and interference in selective attention. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(1), 45–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193146.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fitzgeorge, L., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2011). Recently inhibited responses are avoided for both masked and nonmasked primes in a spatial negative priming task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1435–1452. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0125-7.CrossRef Fitzgeorge, L., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2011). Recently inhibited responses are avoided for both masked and nonmasked primes in a spatial negative priming task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1435–1452. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-011-0125-7.CrossRef
go back to reference Guy, S., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2006). The locus of location repetition latency effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(4), 307–318.PubMedCrossRef Guy, S., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2006). The locus of location repetition latency effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(4), 307–318.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hawley, M. L., Litovsky, R. Y., & Culling, J. F. (2004). The benefit of binaural hearing in a cocktail party: effect of location and type of interferer. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115(2), 833–843. doi:10.1121/1.1639908.PubMedCrossRef Hawley, M. L., Litovsky, R. Y., & Culling, J. F. (2004). The benefit of binaural hearing in a cocktail party: effect of location and type of interferer. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115(2), 833–843. doi:10.​1121/​1.​1639908.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70. Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70.
go back to reference Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory mechanisms of attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic Press. Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory mechanisms of attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for episodic retrieval of inadequate prime responses in auditory negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 932–943. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.932.PubMed Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2006). Evidence for episodic retrieval of inadequate prime responses in auditory negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 932–943. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​32.​4.​932.PubMed
go back to reference Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2013). Intact episodic retrieval in older adults: Evidence from an auditory negative priming task. Experimental Aging Research (in press). Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2013). Intact episodic retrieval in older adults: Evidence from an auditory negative priming task. Experimental Aging Research (in press).
go back to reference Mayr, S., Buchner, A., Möller, M., & Hauke, R. (2011a). Spatial and identity negative priming in audition: Evidence of feature binding in auditory spatial memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(6), 1710–1732. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0138-2.CrossRef Mayr, S., Buchner, A., Möller, M., & Hauke, R. (2011a). Spatial and identity negative priming in audition: Evidence of feature binding in auditory spatial memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(6), 1710–1732. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-011-0138-2.CrossRef
go back to reference Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., & Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(6), 1280–1296. doi:10.3758/BF03212130.CrossRef Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., & Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(6), 1280–1296. doi:10.​3758/​BF03212130.CrossRef
go back to reference Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., & Weaver, B. (1994). Negative priming in a spatial localization task: Feature mismatching and distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 624–646. Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., & Weaver, B. (1994). Negative priming in a spatial localization task: Feature mismatching and distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 624–646.
go back to reference Möller, M., Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2013). Target localization among concurrent sound sources: No evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(1), 132–144. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0380-2.CrossRef Möller, M., Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2013). Target localization among concurrent sound sources: No evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(1), 132–144. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-012-0380-2.CrossRef
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 993–1000. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.993.PubMedCrossRef Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 993–1000. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​18.​5.​993.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Noble, W., & Perrett, S. (2002). Hearing speech against spatially separate competing speech versus competing noise. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(8), 1325–1336. doi:10.3758/BF03194775.CrossRef Noble, W., & Perrett, S. (2002). Hearing speech against spatially separate competing speech versus competing noise. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(8), 1325–1336. doi:10.​3758/​BF03194775.CrossRef
go back to reference Park, J., & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 613–623. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.613.PubMed Park, J., & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 613–623. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​20.​3.​613.PubMed
go back to reference Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 482–495. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.482.PubMedCrossRef Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 482–495. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​31.​3.​482.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shackleton, T. M., Meddis, R., & Hewitt, M. J. (1994). The role of binaural and fundamental frequency difference cues in the identification of concurrently presented vowels. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47A(3), 545–563. doi:10.1080/14640749408401127.CrossRef Shackleton, T. M., Meddis, R., & Hewitt, M. J. (1994). The role of binaural and fundamental frequency difference cues in the identification of concurrently presented vowels. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47A(3), 545–563. doi:10.​1080/​1464074940840112​7.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343. doi:10.1080/713755969.CrossRef Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343. doi:10.​1080/​713755969.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 492–504. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.16.3.492.PubMed Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 492–504. doi:10.​1037/​/​0096-1523.​16.​3.​492.PubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A(4), 591–611. doi:10.1080/14640748508400921.CrossRef Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A(4), 591–611. doi:10.​1080/​1464074850840092​1.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Milliken, B. (1995). Spatial negative priming without mismatching: Comment on Park and Kanwisher (1994). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1220–1229. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1220. Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Milliken, B. (1995). Spatial negative priming without mismatching: Comment on Park and Kanwisher (1994). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1220–1229. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​21.​5.​1220.
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Auditory spatial negative priming: What is remembered of irrelevant sounds and their locations?
Auteurs
Susanne Mayr
Malte Möller
Axel Buchner
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0515-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2014

Psychological Research 3/2014 Naar de uitgave