18-03-2020 | Original Paper | Uitgave 11/2020 Open Access

Attunement in Music Therapy for Young Children with Autism: Revisiting Qualities of Relationship as Mechanisms of Change
- Tijdschrift:
- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders > Uitgave 11/2020
Electronic supplementary material
Publisher's Note
Case: Car Blues with Tom
Within infancy, sensitive attunement between the infant and the primary caregiver creates and shapes relational experiences leading to modes of attachment (Ainsworth et al. 1978; Bowlby 1969; Meins et al. 2001) and further development of social understanding (Stern 1985/2000). Attunement becomes observable through synchronized body movements as well as empathetically coordinated actions, accompanied by affective regulation, sharing and expressions. Through the human capability to detect cross-modal correspondences (Meltzoff 1990) infants are able to synchronize their movements in time, form and intensity with their caregiver and to share inner subjective states with them (Stern 1985/2000; Trevarthen 1998). Successful mutual affective interactions between infant and caregivers, powered by the ability to progress between states of attunement, mis-attunement and re-attunement (Tronick and Cohn 1989), supports the infants’ ability to organize and self-regulate sensorimotor and affective experiences (Stern 1985/2000; Trevarthen and Aitken 2001). At the same time, it strengthens and extends the caregivers` capabilities to affectively engage with the child. Through mutual attunement processes, the infants learn about their agency, develop motivation for co-regulation and gain confidence in mental closeness (Tronick and Beeghly 2011). All of these early achievements promote further social, cognitive, and language development (Greenspan and Shanker 2007). When the infant discovers that the focus of attention, the feeling state, and intention can be shared (Stern 1985/2000) and mismatching affective states can be repaired (Tronick 2008), intersubjectivity can naturally unfold.Tom was heading for his favorite place at the window bench. From there he could see all the cars on the street. He did not show interest in any of the music instruments that were available in the room. Nor, did he show interest in me. I joined him sitting on the bench and looking out of the window, fantasizing that he was fascinated by all those moving cars. Going along with my inner resonance, I started singing about what I could see on the street. A “car blues” emerged. After musically accompanying the moving cars and us watching them for a couple of sessions, I introduced a chorus line to the familiar car blues which was more dynamic and faster in tempo. I imagined that a three-year-old boy might be full of energy and might like to move himself more than he was showing when just sitting on the bench. When hearing the new chorus, Tom jumped from the bench and started spinning around. The music seemed to touch him, somehow woke him up. Immediately, I synchronized my chorus line with his movements, trying to match his pulse and attuning to his dynamic. When exactly meeting his movement with my music, Tom stopped spinning around for some seconds, looked and smiled at me for the first time. (Experience from a music therapy session with “Tom” and the first author.)
Musical and Emotional Attunement
-
Whether the musical and emotional attunement, seen as crucial relational qualities between the music therapist and the child, predicts generalized changes in core autism spectrum traits such as limited communication and social interaction skills.
-
Whether the therapy intensity, i.e. the number of music therapy sessions per week, is associated with the level of attunement achieved.
Methods
Study Design
Participants
Interventions
Assessment: Process Variables
Assessment: Outcome Variables
Statistical Analyses
Results
Sample Characteristics
Characteristics
|
All patients
|
High-intensity MT
3 ×/week
|
Low-intensity MT
1 ×/week
|
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N
|
Value
|
|||||
N
|
Value
|
N
|
Value
|
|||
Age
a
|
101
|
5.4 (5.3, 5.6)
|
51
|
5.4 (5.2, 5.7)
|
50
|
5.4 (5.2, 5.6)
|
Sex (m)
b
|
101
|
85 (84%)
|
51
|
43 (84%)
|
50
|
42 (84%)
|
Diagnosis (ICD-10)
b
|
101
|
51
|
50
|
|||
Asperger's syndrome (F84.5)
|
4 (4%)
|
1 (2%)
|
3 (6%)
|
|||
Childhood autism (F84.0)
|
83 (82%)
|
44 (86%)
|
39 (78%)
|
|||
Pervasive developmental disorder unspecified (F84.9)
|
14 (14%)
|
6 (12%)
|
8 (16%)
|
|||
IQ, standardized test
a
|
57
|
78.8 (72.4, 85.3)
|
28
|
79.6 (68.6, 90.6)
|
29
|
78.1 (70.9, 85.3)
|
Intellectual disability (IQ < 70)
b
|
97
|
40 (41%)
|
49
|
29 (43%)
|
48
|
19 (40%)
|
ADOS module
b
|
101
|
51
|
50
|
|||
Module 1
|
61 (60%)
|
33 (65%)
|
28 (56%)
|
|||
Module 2
|
39 (39%)
|
18 (35%)
|
21 (42%)
|
|||
Module 3
|
1 (1%)
|
0 (0%)
|
1 (2%)
|
|||
ADOS
a
|
||||||
Total score
|
101
|
18.3 (17.3, 19.3)
|
51
|
18.8 (17.4, 20.2)
|
50
|
17.8 (16.2, 19.3)
|
Social affect
|
101
|
14.4 (13.5, 15.2)
|
51
|
15 (13.7, 16.2)
|
50
|
13.8 (12.6, 14.9)
|
Language and communication
|
101
|
3.5 (3.2, 3.8)
|
51
|
3.7 (3.2, 4.1)
|
50
|
3.2 (2.8, 3.7)
|
Reciprocal social interaction
|
101
|
10.9 (10.2, 11.6)
|
51
|
11.3 (10.3, 12.3)
|
50
|
10.5 (9.6, 11.5)
|
Restricted and repetitive behavior
|
101
|
3.9 (3.5, 4.3)
|
51
|
3.8 (3.3, 4.4)
|
50
|
4 (3.4, 4.6)
|
SRS
a
|
||||||
Total score
|
101
|
96.1 (90.5, 101.7)
|
51
|
94.5 (87.5, 101.6)
|
50
|
97.6 (88.9, 106.3)
|
Awareness
|
101
|
12.2 (11.4, 13)
|
51
|
12 (11, 13)
|
50
|
12.4 (11.2, 13.7)
|
Cognition
|
101
|
18.2 (17, 19.4)
|
51
|
18 (16.5, 19.5)
|
50
|
18.4 (16.5, 20.4)
|
Communication
|
101
|
32.1 (30.1, 34.1)
|
51
|
31.6 (29, 34.3)
|
50
|
32.6 (29.4, 35.7)
|
Motivation
|
101
|
15.1 (14.1, 16.2)
|
51
|
14.9 (13.5, 16.3)
|
50
|
15.4 (13.8, 16.9)
|
Mannerisms
|
101
|
18.4 (17, 19.9)
|
51
|
18.1 (16.2, 20)
|
50
|
18.8 (16.7, 20.9)
|
Descriptive Analysis of AQR Modi
Linear Mixed-Effects Models
B
|
95%CI
|
df
|
t
|
p-value
|
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ADOS total
|
|||||
AQR match rate
|
− 9.24
|
(− 13.30, − 5.18)
|
99
|
− 4.47
|
< .0001
|
AQR match rate × (5 months vs. BL)
|
0.44
|
(− 2.61, 3.49)
|
194
|
0.28
|
0.7779
|
AQR match rate × (12 months vs. BL)
|
− 0.62
|
(− 3.73, 2.48)
|
194
|
− 0.39
|
0.6966
|
ADOS social affect
|
|||||
AQR match rate
|
− 7.21
|
(− 10.59, − 3.83)
|
99
|
− 4.19
|
0.0001
|
AQR match rate × (5 months vs. BL)
|
0.40
|
(− 2.23, 3.03)
|
194
|
0.30
|
0.7673
|
AQR match rate × (12 months vs. BL)
|
− 0.36
|
(− 3.04, 2.32)
|
194
|
− 0.26
|
0.7915
|
ADOS restricted and repetitive behavior
|
|||||
AQR match rate
|
− 2.03
|
(− 3.59, − 0.48)
|
99
|
− 2.58
|
0.0115
|
AQR match rate × (5 months vs. BL)
|
0.04
|
(− 1.43, 1.52)
|
194
|
0.06
|
0.9555
|
AQR match rate × (12 months vs. BL)
|
− 0.26
|
(− 1.76, 1.24)
|
194
|
− 0.34
|
0.7331
|
SRS total
|
|||||
AQR match rate
|
− 13.49
|
(− 36.28, 9.31)
|
99
|
− 1.16
|
0.2483
|
AQR match rate × (5 months vs. BL)
|
− 16.06
|
(− 31.85, − 0.27)
|
169
|
− 1.99
|
0.0486
|
AQR match rate × (12 months vs. BL)
|
− 3.24
|
(− 20.03, 13.56)
|
169
|
− 0.38
|
0.7072
|