Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2016

10-09-2015 | Original Article

Attentional biases in ruminators and worriers

Auteurs: Mieke Beckwé, Natacha Deroost

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study aims to investigate whether attentional biases typically associated with depression and anxiety already exist on a sub-clinical level. A transdiagnostic characteristic, both affective disorders have in common at a sub-clinical level, is persistent negative thinking (PNT), called rumination in depression and worrying in anxiety disorders. We investigated the association between these two types of PNT and attentional biases, using two different versions of the exogenous cueing tasks (ECT) in two different experiments. In Experiment 1, the cues of the ECT were negative and positive personality traits. This allowed us to investigate whether high-ruminators (N = 29), analogous to depressed patients, have difficulties to disengage attention from negative personality traits, as compared to low-ruminators (N = 40). In Experiment 2, the cues of the ECT were negative words related to themes participants frequently worry about versus positive words. This was done to investigate whether high-worriers (N = 26), analogous to anxious persons, have a strong tendency to automatically direct attention toward worry-related information, as compared to low-worriers (N = 27). The results of Experiment 1 showed that high-ruminators have difficulties to disengage their attention from negative personality traits. The results of Experiment 2 indicated that there were no attentional biases for high-worriers. These results show that the attentional bias typically associated with depression is already present at a sub-clinical level, whereas this seems not to be the case for the attentional bias typically associated with anxiety.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Correlation between raw RRS and PSWQ scores: r = .644, p < .001, overlap between groups, divided based on median split: 50 % (31 out of 62 students score low on both RRS and PSWQ or high on both RRS and PSWQ).
 
2
None of the participants did not indicate any worry-theme as being relevant; two participants indicated that all of the worry-themes were personally relevant for them. On average, respondents indicated that two out of the four worry-themes were personally relevant to them.
 
3
Correlation between raw RRS and PSWQ scores: r = .704, p < .001, overlap between groups, divided based on median split: 53 % (28 out of 53 students score low on both RRS and PSWQ or high on both RRS and PSWQ).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ansseau, M., Fischler, B., Dierick, M., Albert, A., Leyman, S., & Mignon, A. (2008). Socioeconomic correlates of generalized anxiety disorder and major depression in primary care: the GADIS II study (Generalized Anxiety and Depression Impact Survey II). Depression and Anxiety, 25(6), 506–513.CrossRefPubMed Ansseau, M., Fischler, B., Dierick, M., Albert, A., Leyman, S., & Mignon, A. (2008). Socioeconomic correlates of generalized anxiety disorder and major depression in primary care: the GADIS II study (Generalized Anxiety and Depression Impact Survey II). Depression and Anxiety, 25(6), 506–513.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non anxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.CrossRefPubMed Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non anxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Beckwé, M., Deroost, N., Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychological Research, 1–10. Beckwé, M., Deroost, N., Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychological Research, 1–10.
go back to reference Borkovec, T. D., & Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 153–158.CrossRef Borkovec, T. D., & Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 153–158.CrossRef
go back to reference Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12(6), 737–753.CrossRef Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12(6), 737–753.CrossRef
go back to reference De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Goubert, L., Onraedt, T., Vanderhasselt, M., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Impaired cognitive control moderates the association between stress and rumination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 519–525.CrossRefPubMed De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Goubert, L., Onraedt, T., Vanderhasselt, M., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Impaired cognitive control moderates the association between stress and rumination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 519–525.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 50–70.CrossRef De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 50–70.CrossRef
go back to reference Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11), 2664–2678.CrossRefPubMed Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11), 2664–2678.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: the role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63.CrossRef Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: the role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63.CrossRef
go back to reference Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16(3), 355–379.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16(3), 355–379.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., … et al. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68(11), 982–990.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., … et al. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68(11), 982–990.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hermans, D., & De Houwer, J. (1994). Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 Dutch words. Psychologica Belgica, 34, 115–140. Hermans, D., & De Houwer, J. (1994). Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 Dutch words. Psychologica Belgica, 34, 115–140.
go back to reference Kerkhof, A., Hermans, D., Figee, A., Laeremans, I., Pieters, G., & Aardema, A. (2000). De Penn State Worry Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: Eerste resultaten bij Nederlandse en Vlaamse klinische en poliklinische populaties (The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: first results in Dutch and Flemish clinical populations). Gedragstherapie, 33, 135–146. Kerkhof, A., Hermans, D., Figee, A., Laeremans, I., Pieters, G., & Aardema, A. (2000). De Penn State Worry Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: Eerste resultaten bij Nederlandse en Vlaamse klinische en poliklinische populaties (The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: first results in Dutch and Flemish clinical populations). Gedragstherapie, 33, 135–146.
go back to reference Kessler, R. C., Gruber, M., Hettema, J. M., Hwang, I., Sampson, N., & Yonkers, K. A. (2008). Co-morbid major depression and generalized anxiety disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 38(3), 365.CrossRefPubMed Kessler, R. C., Gruber, M., Hettema, J. M., Hwang, I., Sampson, N., & Yonkers, K. A. (2008). Co-morbid major depression and generalized anxiety disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 38(3), 365.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1757–1771.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1757–1771.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 219–225.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 219–225.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5(3), 365.CrossRefPubMed Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5(3), 365.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107.CrossRefPubMed MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495.CrossRef Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495.CrossRef
go back to reference Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(9), 809–848.CrossRefPubMed Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(9), 809–848.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29–45.CrossRef Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.CrossRefPubMed Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oathes, D. J., Siegle, G. J., & Ray, W. J. (2011). Chronic worry and the temporal dynamics of emotional processing. Emotion, 11(1), 101.CrossRefPubMed Oathes, D. J., Siegle, G. J., & Ray, W. J. (2011). Chronic worry and the temporal dynamics of emotional processing. Emotion, 11(1), 101.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ormel, J., Von Korff, M., Ustun, T. B., Pini, S., Korten, A., & Oldehinkel, T. (1994). Common mental disorders and disability: a strong and cross-culturally consistent relationship. Results from the WHO Collaborative Study. JAMA, 272, 1741–1748.CrossRefPubMed Ormel, J., Von Korff, M., Ustun, T. B., Pini, S., Korten, A., & Oldehinkel, T. (1994). Common mental disorders and disability: a strong and cross-culturally consistent relationship. Results from the WHO Collaborative Study. JAMA, 272, 1741–1748.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and performance X: control of language processes, 32, 531–556. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and performance X: control of language processes, 32, 531–556.
go back to reference Pratt, P., Tallis, F., & Eysenck, M. (1997). Information-processing, storage characteristics and worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(11), 1015–1023.CrossRefPubMed Pratt, P., Tallis, F., & Eysenck, M. (1997). Information-processing, storage characteristics and worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(11), 1015–1023.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Hoes, D., Hermans, D., Van Den Eede, F., & Franck, E. (2009). ‘Reflection’ en ‘brooding’ als subtypes van rumineren: eenherziening van de Ruminative Response Scale (‘reflection’ and ‘brooding’ as subtypes of rumination: a revision of the ruminative response scale). Gedragstherapie, 42(3/4), 205–214. Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Hoes, D., Hermans, D., Van Den Eede, F., & Franck, E. (2009). ‘Reflection’ en ‘brooding’ als subtypes van rumineren: eenherziening van de Ruminative Response Scale (‘reflection’ and ‘brooding’ as subtypes of rumination: a revision of the ruminative response scale). Gedragstherapie, 42(3/4), 205–214.
go back to reference Reidy, J., & Richards, A. (1997). A memory bias for threat in high-trait anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(4), 653–663.CrossRef Reidy, J., & Richards, A. (1997). A memory bias for threat in high-trait anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(4), 653–663.CrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
go back to reference Segal, Z. V., Gemar, M., Truchon, C., Guirguis, M., & Horowitz, L. M. (1995). A priming methodology for studying self-representation in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 205.CrossRefPubMed Segal, Z. V., Gemar, M., Truchon, C., Guirguis, M., & Horowitz, L. M. (1995). A priming methodology for studying self-representation in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 205.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C., Alden, L. E., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and rumination: repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(6), 671–688.CrossRef Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C., Alden, L. E., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and rumination: repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(6), 671–688.CrossRef
go back to reference Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Carter, C. S., Ramel, W., & Thase, M. E. (2003). Do the seconds turn into hours? Relationships between sustained pupil dilation in response to emotional information and self-reported rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 365–382.CrossRef Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Carter, C. S., Ramel, W., & Thase, M. E. (2003). Do the seconds turn into hours? Relationships between sustained pupil dilation in response to emotional information and self-reported rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 365–382.CrossRef
go back to reference Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
go back to reference Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.CrossRef Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Van der Does, A. J. W. (2002). Handleiding: De Nederlandse versie van de Beck Depression Inventory, 2de Editie (the Dutch version of the Beck Depression Inventory (2nd ed.). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger B.V. Van der Does, A. J. W. (2002). Handleiding: De Nederlandse versie van de Beck Depression Inventory, 2de Editie (the Dutch version of the Beck Depression Inventory (2nd ed.). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger B.V.
go back to reference Van der Ploeg, H. M. (2000). Handleiding bij de Zelf-Beoordelings Vragenlijst (the Dutch version of the STAI). Amsterdam: Harcourt Assessment B.V. Van der Ploeg, H. M. (2000). Handleiding bij de Zelf-Beoordelings Vragenlijst (the Dutch version of the STAI). Amsterdam: Harcourt Assessment B.V.
go back to reference Van Rijsoort, S., Emmelkamp, P., & Vervaeke, G. (1999). The Penn StateWorry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: structure, reliability and validity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 297–307.CrossRef Van Rijsoort, S., Emmelkamp, P., & Vervaeke, G. (1999). The Penn StateWorry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: structure, reliability and validity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 297–307.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Rijsoort, S. N., Vervaeke, G. A. C., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1997). De Penn State Worry Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: eerste resultaten bij een normale Nederlandse populatie (The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: first results in a Dutch population). Gedragstherapie, 30, 121–128. Van Rijsoort, S. N., Vervaeke, G. A. C., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1997). De Penn State Worry Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: eerste resultaten bij een normale Nederlandse populatie (The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: first results in a Dutch population). Gedragstherapie, 30, 121–128.
go back to reference Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Putman, P., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Interacting effects of worry and anxiety on attentional disengagement from threat. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(2), 146–152.CrossRefPubMed Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Putman, P., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Interacting effects of worry and anxiety on attentional disengagement from threat. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(2), 146–152.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wicks-Nelson, R., & Israel, A. C. (2009). Abnormal child and adolescent psychology. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall. Wicks-Nelson, R., & Israel, A. C. (2009). Abnormal child and adolescent psychology. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall.
go back to reference Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(3), 665–681.CrossRef Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(3), 665–681.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Attentional biases in ruminators and worriers
Auteurs
Mieke Beckwé
Natacha Deroost
Publicatiedatum
10-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0703-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2016

Psychological Research 6/2016 Naar de uitgave