Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2014

01-05-2014 | Review

Attention to memory: orienting attention to sound object representations

Auteurs: Kristina C. Backer, Claude Alain

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Despite a growing acceptance that attention and memory interact, and that attention can be focused on an active internal mental representation (i.e., reflective attention), there has been a paucity of work focusing on reflective attention to ‘sound objects’ (i.e., mental representations of actual sound sources in the environment). Further research on the dynamic interactions between auditory attention and memory, as well as its degree of neuroplasticity, is important for understanding how sound objects are represented, maintained, and accessed in the brain. This knowledge can then guide the development of training programs to help individuals with attention and memory problems. This review article focuses on attention to memory with an emphasis on behavioral and neuroimaging studies that have begun to explore the mechanisms that mediate reflective attentional orienting in vision and more recently, in audition. Reflective attention refers to situations in which attention is oriented toward internal representations rather than focused on external stimuli. We propose four general principles underlying attention to short-term memory. Furthermore, we suggest that mechanisms involved in orienting attention to visual object representations may also apply for orienting attention to sound object representations.
Voetnoten
1
Recall that we are using the term “perceptual” to refer to attention to ongoing, external stimuli (per Chun & Johnson, 2011).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Abramovitch, A., Dar, R., Hermesh, H., & Schweiger, A. (2012). Comparative neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: implications for a novel executive overload model of OCD. Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(2), 161–191. doi:10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02021.x.PubMed Abramovitch, A., Dar, R., Hermesh, H., & Schweiger, A. (2012). Comparative neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: implications for a novel executive overload model of OCD. Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(2), 161–191. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1748-6653.​2011.​02021.​x.PubMed
go back to reference Alain, C., Achim, A., & Richer, F. (1993). Perceptual context and the selective attention effect on auditory event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 30(6), 572–580.PubMed Alain, C., Achim, A., & Richer, F. (1993). Perceptual context and the selective attention effect on auditory event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 30(6), 572–580.PubMed
go back to reference Alain, C., & Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, D202–D212.PubMed Alain, C., & Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, D202–D212.PubMed
go back to reference Alain, C., & Bernstein, L. J. (2008). From sounds to meaning: the role of attention during auditory scene analysis. Current Opinion in Otolaryngology & Head and Neck Surgery, 16, 485–489. Alain, C., & Bernstein, L. J. (2008). From sounds to meaning: the role of attention during auditory scene analysis. Current Opinion in Otolaryngology & Head and Neck Surgery, 16, 485–489.
go back to reference Alain, C., & Ross, B. (2008). The role of neuroelectric and neuromagnetic recordings in assessing learning and rehabilitation effects. In D. T. Stuss & G. Winocur (Eds.), Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, Evidence and Applications (2nd ed., pp. 183–199). New York: Cambridge University Press. Alain, C., & Ross, B. (2008). The role of neuroelectric and neuromagnetic recordings in assessing learning and rehabilitation effects. In D. T. Stuss & G. Winocur (Eds.), Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, Evidence and Applications (2nd ed., pp. 183–199). New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Alain, C., & Woods, D. L. (1993). Distractor clustering enhances detection speed and accuracy during selective listening. Perception & Psychophysics, 54(4), 509–514. Alain, C., & Woods, D. L. (1993). Distractor clustering enhances detection speed and accuracy during selective listening. Perception & Psychophysics, 54(4), 509–514.
go back to reference Alain, C., & Woods, D. L. (1994). Signal clustering modulates auditory cortical activity in humans. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(5), 501–516. Alain, C., & Woods, D. L. (1994). Signal clustering modulates auditory cortical activity in humans. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(5), 501–516.
go back to reference Alho, K., Donauer, N., Paavilainen, P., Reinikainen, K., Sams, M., & Naatanen, R. (1987a). Stimulus selection during auditory spatial attention as expressed by event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 24(2), 153–162.PubMed Alho, K., Donauer, N., Paavilainen, P., Reinikainen, K., Sams, M., & Naatanen, R. (1987a). Stimulus selection during auditory spatial attention as expressed by event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 24(2), 153–162.PubMed
go back to reference Alho, K., Tottola, K., Reinikainen, K., Sams, M., & Naatanen, R. (1987b). Brain mechanism of selective listening reflected by event-related potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 68(6), 458–470.PubMed Alho, K., Tottola, K., Reinikainen, K., Sams, M., & Naatanen, R. (1987b). Brain mechanism of selective listening reflected by event-related potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 68(6), 458–470.PubMed
go back to reference Amir, N., Cashman, L., & Foa, E. B. (1997). Strategies of thought control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 775–777.PubMed Amir, N., Cashman, L., & Foa, E. B. (1997). Strategies of thought control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 775–777.PubMed
go back to reference Armstrong, T., Sarawgi, S., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Attentional bias toward threat in contamination fear: overt components and behavioral correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 232–237. doi:10.1037/a0024453.PubMedCentralPubMed Armstrong, T., Sarawgi, S., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Attentional bias toward threat in contamination fear: overt components and behavioral correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 232–237. doi:10.​1037/​a0024453.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Arnott, S. R., & Alain, C. (2002). Effects of perceptual context on event-related brain potentials during auditory spatial attention. Psychophysiology, 39(5), 625–632.PubMed Arnott, S. R., & Alain, C. (2002). Effects of perceptual context on event-related brain potentials during auditory spatial attention. Psychophysiology, 39(5), 625–632.PubMed
go back to reference Arnsten, A. F., & Rubia, K. (2012). Neurobiological circuits regulating attention, cognitive control, motivation, and emotion: disruptions in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(4), 356–367. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.008.PubMed Arnsten, A. F., & Rubia, K. (2012). Neurobiological circuits regulating attention, cognitive control, motivation, and emotion: disruptions in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(4), 356–367. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jaac.​2012.​01.​008.PubMed
go back to reference Astle, D. E., Summerfield, J., Griffin, I., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Orienting attention to locations in mental representations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(1), 146–162. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0218-3. Astle, D. E., Summerfield, J., Griffin, I., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Orienting attention to locations in mental representations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(1), 146–162. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-011-0218-3.
go back to reference Axmacher, N., Mormann, F., Fernandez, G., Cohen, M. X., Elger, C. E., & Fell, J. (2007). Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(29), 7807–7816. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0962-07.2007. Axmacher, N., Mormann, F., Fernandez, G., Cohen, M. X., Elger, C. E., & Fell, J. (2007). Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(29), 7807–7816. doi:10.​1523/​JNEUROSCI.​0962-07.​2007.
go back to reference Backer, K. C., & Alain, C. (2012). Orienting attention to sound object representations attenuates change deafness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1554–1566. doi:10.1037/a0027858.PubMed Backer, K. C., & Alain, C. (2012). Orienting attention to sound object representations attenuates change deafness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1554–1566. doi:10.​1037/​a0027858.PubMed
go back to reference Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.PubMed Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.PubMed
go back to reference Banerjee, S., Snyder, A. C., Molholm, S., & Foxe, J. J. (2011). Oscillatory alpha-band mechanisms and the deployment of spatial attention to anticipated auditory and visual target locations: supramodal or sensory-specific control mechanisms? The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(27), 9923–9932. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4660-10.2011. Banerjee, S., Snyder, A. C., Molholm, S., & Foxe, J. J. (2011). Oscillatory alpha-band mechanisms and the deployment of spatial attention to anticipated auditory and visual target locations: supramodal or sensory-specific control mechanisms? The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(27), 9923–9932. doi:10.​1523/​JNEUROSCI.​4660-10.​2011.
go back to reference Bastiaansen, M., Magyari, L., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Syntactic unification operations are reflected in oscillatory dynamics during on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(7), 1333–1347. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21283.PubMed Bastiaansen, M., Magyari, L., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Syntactic unification operations are reflected in oscillatory dynamics during on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(7), 1333–1347. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2009.​21283.PubMed
go back to reference Bastiaansen, M. C., Oostenveld, R., Jensen, O., & Hagoort, P. (2008). I see what you mean: theta power increases are involved in the retrieval of lexical semantic information. Brain and Language, 106(1), 15–28. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.006.PubMed Bastiaansen, M. C., Oostenveld, R., Jensen, O., & Hagoort, P. (2008). I see what you mean: theta power increases are involved in the retrieval of lexical semantic information. Brain and Language, 106(1), 15–28. doi:10.​1016/​j.​bandl.​2007.​10.​006.PubMed
go back to reference Bastiaansen, M. C., van der Linden, M., Ter Keurs, M., Dijkstra, T., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Theta responses are involved in lexical-semantic retrieval during language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 530–541. doi:10.1162/0898929053279469.PubMed Bastiaansen, M. C., van der Linden, M., Ter Keurs, M., Dijkstra, T., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Theta responses are involved in lexical-semantic retrieval during language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 530–541. doi:10.​1162/​0898929053279469​.PubMed
go back to reference Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(3), 451–470.PubMed Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(3), 451–470.PubMed
go back to reference Bertrand, O., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2000). Oscillatory gamma activity in humans: a possible role for object representation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38(3), 211–223.PubMed Bertrand, O., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2000). Oscillatory gamma activity in humans: a possible role for object representation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38(3), 211–223.PubMed
go back to reference Best, V., Gallun, F. J., Ihlefeld, A., & Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2006). The influence of spatial separation on divided listening. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120(3), 1506–1516.PubMed Best, V., Gallun, F. J., Ihlefeld, A., & Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2006). The influence of spatial separation on divided listening. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120(3), 1506–1516.PubMed
go back to reference Brefczynski, J. A., & DeYoe, E. A. (1999). A physiological correlate of the ‘spotlight’ of visual attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 370–374.PubMed Brefczynski, J. A., & DeYoe, E. A. (1999). A physiological correlate of the ‘spotlight’ of visual attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 370–374.PubMed
go back to reference Buchsbaum, B. R., Olsen, R. K., Koch, P., & Berman, K. F. (2005). Human dorsal and ventral auditory streams subserve rehearsal-based and echoic processes during verbal working memory. Neuron, 48(4), 687–697. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.029. Buchsbaum, B. R., Olsen, R. K., Koch, P., & Berman, K. F. (2005). Human dorsal and ventral auditory streams subserve rehearsal-based and echoic processes during verbal working memory. Neuron, 48(4), 687–697. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuron.​2005.​09.​029.
go back to reference Cabeza, R., Mazuz, Y. S., Stokes, J., Kragel, J. E., Woldorff, M. G., Ciaramelli, E., et al. (2011). Overlapping parietal activity in memory and perception: evidence for the attention to memory model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3209–3217. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00065.PubMedCentralPubMed Cabeza, R., Mazuz, Y. S., Stokes, J., Kragel, J. E., Woldorff, M. G., Ciaramelli, E., et al. (2011). Overlapping parietal activity in memory and perception: evidence for the attention to memory model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3209–3217. doi:10.​1162/​jocn_​a_​00065.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Cashdollar, N., Malecki, U., Rugg-Gunn, F. J., Duncan, J. S., Lavie, N., & Duzel, E. (2009). Hippocampus-dependent and -independent theta-networks of active maintenance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(48), 20493–20498. doi:10.1073/pnas.0904823106.PubMedCentralPubMed Cashdollar, N., Malecki, U., Rugg-Gunn, F. J., Duncan, J. S., Lavie, N., & Duzel, E. (2009). Hippocampus-dependent and -independent theta-networks of active maintenance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(48), 20493–20498. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​0904823106.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Chen, Z., & Cave, K. R. (2008). Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(8), 1435–1443. Chen, Z., & Cave, K. R. (2008). Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(8), 1435–1443.
go back to reference Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with two ears. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979. Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with two ears. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 292–297. doi:10.1038/73009.PubMed Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 292–297. doi:10.​1038/​73009.PubMed
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 508–523. doi:10.1162/089892902317362029.PubMed Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 508–523. doi:10.​1162/​0898929023173620​29.PubMed
go back to reference Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. doi:10.1038/nrn755.PubMed Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. doi:10.​1038/​nrn755.PubMed
go back to reference Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–114; discussion 114–185. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–114; discussion 114–185.
go back to reference Cusack, R., Carlyon, R. P., & Robertson, I. H. (2000). Neglect between but not within auditory objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1056–1065.PubMed Cusack, R., Carlyon, R. P., & Robertson, I. H. (2000). Neglect between but not within auditory objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1056–1065.PubMed
go back to reference Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review, 87(3), 272–300.PubMed Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review, 87(3), 272–300.PubMed
go back to reference Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(4), 501–517. Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(4), 501–517.
go back to reference Dyson, B. J., Alain, C., & He, Y. (2005). Effects of visual attentional load on low-level auditory scene analysis. Cognitive Affective Behaviorial Neuroscience, 5(3), 319–338. Dyson, B. J., Alain, C., & He, Y. (2005). Effects of visual attentional load on low-level auditory scene analysis. Cognitive Affective Behaviorial Neuroscience, 5(3), 319–338.
go back to reference Dyson, B. J., & Ishfaq, F. (2008). Auditory memory can be object based. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 409–412. Dyson, B. J., & Ishfaq, F. (2008). Auditory memory can be object based. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 409–412.
go back to reference Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 161–177. Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 161–177.
go back to reference Eramudugolla, R., Irvine, D. R., McAnally, K. I., Martin, R. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2005). Directed attention eliminates ‘change deafness’ in complex auditory scenes. Current Biology, 15(12), 1108–1113.PubMed Eramudugolla, R., Irvine, D. R., McAnally, K. I., Martin, R. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2005). Directed attention eliminates ‘change deafness’ in complex auditory scenes. Current Biology, 15(12), 1108–1113.PubMed
go back to reference Giesbrecht, B., Woldorff, M. G., Song, A. W., & Mangun, G. R. (2003). Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention. Neuroimage, 19(3), 496–512.PubMed Giesbrecht, B., Woldorff, M. G., Song, A. W., & Mangun, G. R. (2003). Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention. Neuroimage, 19(3), 496–512.PubMed
go back to reference Grillon, M. L., Johnson, M. K., Krebs, M. O., & Huron, C. (2008). Comparing effects of perceptual and reflective repetition on subjective experience during later recognition memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 753–764. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.09.004.PubMed Grillon, M. L., Johnson, M. K., Krebs, M. O., & Huron, C. (2008). Comparing effects of perceptual and reflective repetition on subjective experience during later recognition memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 753–764. doi:10.​1016/​j.​concog.​2007.​09.​004.PubMed
go back to reference Hansen, J. C., & Hillyard, S. A. (1983). Selective attention to multidimensional auditory stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(1), 1–19.PubMed Hansen, J. C., & Hillyard, S. A. (1983). Selective attention to multidimensional auditory stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(1), 1–19.PubMed
go back to reference Hecht, L. N., Abbs, B., & Vecera, S. P. (2008). Auditory object-based attention. Visual Cognition, 16, 1109–1115. Hecht, L. N., Abbs, B., & Vecera, S. P. (2008). Auditory object-based attention. Visual Cognition, 16, 1109–1115.
go back to reference Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science, 182(108), 177–180.PubMed Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science, 182(108), 177–180.PubMed
go back to reference Hopf, J. M., Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., Hagner, T., Mangun, G. R., Scheich, H., et al. (2000). Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. Cerebral Cortex, 10(12), 1233–1241.PubMed Hopf, J. M., Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., Hagner, T., Mangun, G. R., Scheich, H., et al. (2000). Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. Cerebral Cortex, 10(12), 1233–1241.PubMed
go back to reference Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284–291. doi:10.1038/72999.PubMed Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284–291. doi:10.​1038/​72999.PubMed
go back to reference Hubner, R., & Hafter, E. R. (1995). Cuing mechanisms in auditory signal detection. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(2), 197–202. Hubner, R., & Hafter, E. R. (1995). Cuing mechanisms in auditory signal detection. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(2), 197–202.
go back to reference Jensen, O., Gelfand, J., Kounios, J., & Lisman, J. E. (2002). Oscillations in the alpha band (9–12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. Cerebral Cortex, 12(8), 877–882.PubMed Jensen, O., Gelfand, J., Kounios, J., & Lisman, J. E. (2002). Oscillations in the alpha band (9–12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. Cerebral Cortex, 12(8), 877–882.PubMed
go back to reference Johnson, M. K., & Hirst, W. (1993). MEM: Memory subsystems as processes. In A. A. Collins, S. S. Gathercole, M. M. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of Memory. East Sussex: Erlbaum. Johnson, M. K., & Hirst, W. (1993). MEM: Memory subsystems as processes. In A. A. Collins, S. S. Gathercole, M. M. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of Memory. East Sussex: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Johnson, M. R., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Top-down enhancement and suppression of activity in category-selective extrastriate cortex from an act of reflective attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(12), 2320–2327. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.21183. Johnson, M. R., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Top-down enhancement and suppression of activity in category-selective extrastriate cortex from an act of reflective attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(12), 2320–2327. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2008.​21183.
go back to reference Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Greene, E. J., Cunningham, W. A., & Sanislow, C. A. (2005). Using fMRI to investigate a component process of reflection: prefrontal correlates of refreshing a just-activated representation. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(3), 339–361. Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Greene, E. J., Cunningham, W. A., & Sanislow, C. A. (2005). Using fMRI to investigate a component process of reflection: prefrontal correlates of refreshing a just-activated representation. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(3), 339–361.
go back to reference Jokisch, D., & Jensen, O. (2007). Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(12), 3244–3251. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5399-06.2007. Jokisch, D., & Jensen, O. (2007). Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(12), 3244–3251. doi:10.​1523/​JNEUROSCI.​5399-06.​2007.
go back to reference Kaiser, J., & Bertrand, O. (2003). Dynamics of working memory for moving sounds: an event-related potential and scalp current density study. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1427–1438.PubMed Kaiser, J., & Bertrand, O. (2003). Dynamics of working memory for moving sounds: an event-related potential and scalp current density study. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1427–1438.PubMed
go back to reference Kaiser, J., Lutzenberger, W., Decker, C., Wibral, M., & Rahm, B. (2009a). Task- and performance-related modulation of domain-specific auditory short-term memory representations in the gamma-band. Neuroimage, 46(4), 1127–1136. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.011.PubMed Kaiser, J., Lutzenberger, W., Decker, C., Wibral, M., & Rahm, B. (2009a). Task- and performance-related modulation of domain-specific auditory short-term memory representations in the gamma-band. Neuroimage, 46(4), 1127–1136. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuroimage.​2009.​03.​011.PubMed
go back to reference Kaiser, J., Ripper, B., Birbaumer, N., & Lutzenberger, W. (2003). Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. Neuroimage, 20(2), 816–827. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00350-1.PubMed Kaiser, J., Ripper, B., Birbaumer, N., & Lutzenberger, W. (2003). Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. Neuroimage, 20(2), 816–827. doi:10.​1016/​S1053-8119(03)00350-1.PubMed
go back to reference Kerlin, J. R., Shahin, A. J., & Miller, L. M. (2010). Attentional gain control of ongoing cortical speech representations in a “cocktail party”. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(2), 620–628.PubMedCentralPubMed Kerlin, J. R., Shahin, A. J., & Miller, L. M. (2010). Attentional gain control of ongoing cortical speech representations in a “cocktail party”. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(2), 620–628.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., & De Jong, P. J. (2009). Negative information enhances the attentional blink in dysphoria. Depress Anxiety, 26(1), E16–E22. doi:10.1002/da.20420.PubMed Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., & De Jong, P. J. (2009). Negative information enhances the attentional blink in dysphoria. Depress Anxiety, 26(1), E16–E22. doi:10.​1002/​da.​20420.PubMed
go back to reference Krumbholz, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Fink, G. R. (2007). Feature- and object-based attentional modulation in the human auditory “where” pathway. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(10), 1721–1733.PubMed Krumbholz, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Fink, G. R. (2007). Feature- and object-based attentional modulation in the human auditory “where” pathway. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(10), 1721–1733.PubMed
go back to reference LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(3), 371–379.PubMed LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(3), 371–379.PubMed
go back to reference Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. (2003). Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149–164. [pii]: S0042698902004029.PubMed Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. (2003). Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149–164. [pii]: S0042698902004029.PubMed
go back to reference Lange, K. (2013). The ups and downs of temporal orienting: a review of auditory temporal orienting studies and a model associating the heterogeneous findings on the auditory N1 with opposite effects of attention and prediction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 263. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00263.PubMedCentralPubMed Lange, K. (2013). The ups and downs of temporal orienting: a review of auditory temporal orienting studies and a model associating the heterogeneous findings on the auditory N1 with opposite effects of attention and prediction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 263. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2013.​00263.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Lutzenberger, W., Ripper, B., Busse, L., Birbaumer, N., & Kaiser, J. (2002). Dynamics of gamma-band activity during an audiospatial working memory task in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(13), 5630–5638. [pii]: 2002657022/13/5630.PubMed Lutzenberger, W., Ripper, B., Busse, L., Birbaumer, N., & Kaiser, J. (2002). Dynamics of gamma-band activity during an audiospatial working memory task in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(13), 5630–5638. [pii]: 2002657022/13/5630.PubMed
go back to reference Makovski, T., Sussman, R., & Jiang, Y. V. (2008). Orienting attention in visual working memory reduces interference from memory probes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 369–380. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.369.PubMed Makovski, T., Sussman, R., & Jiang, Y. V. (2008). Orienting attention in visual working memory reduces interference from memory probes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 369–380. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​34.​2.​369.PubMed
go back to reference Matsukura, M., Luck, S. J., & Vecera, S. P. (2007). Attention effects during visual short-term memory maintenance: protection or prioritization? Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 1422–1434. Matsukura, M., Luck, S. J., & Vecera, S. P. (2007). Attention effects during visual short-term memory maintenance: protection or prioritization? Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 1422–1434.
go back to reference Mayer, A. R., Harrington, D. L., Stephen, J., Adair, J. C., & Lee, R. R. (2007). An event-related fMRI Study of exogenous facilitation and inhibition of return in the auditory modality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 455–467. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.455.PubMed Mayer, A. R., Harrington, D. L., Stephen, J., Adair, J. C., & Lee, R. R. (2007). An event-related fMRI Study of exogenous facilitation and inhibition of return in the auditory modality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 455–467. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2007.​19.​3.​455.PubMed
go back to reference McMains, S. A., & Somers, D. C. (2004). Multiple spotlights of attentional selection in human visual cortex. Neuron, 42(4), 677–686.PubMed McMains, S. A., & Somers, D. C. (2004). Multiple spotlights of attentional selection in human visual cortex. Neuron, 42(4), 677–686.PubMed
go back to reference McNeely, H. E., Lau, M. A., Christensen, B. K., & Alain, C. (2008). Neurophysiological evidence of cognitive inhibition anomalies in persons with major depressive disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(7), 1578–1589.PubMed McNeely, H. E., Lau, M. A., Christensen, B. K., & Alain, C. (2008). Neurophysiological evidence of cognitive inhibition anomalies in persons with major depressive disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(7), 1578–1589.PubMed
go back to reference Mondor, T. A., & Amirault, K. J. (1998). Effect of same- and different-modality spatial cues on auditory and visual target identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 745–755.PubMed Mondor, T. A., & Amirault, K. J. (1998). Effect of same- and different-modality spatial cues on auditory and visual target identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 745–755.PubMed
go back to reference Muller, N., & Weisz, N. (2012). Lateralized auditory cortical alpha band activity and interregional connectivity pattern reflect anticipation of target sounds. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1604–1613. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr232.PubMed Muller, N., & Weisz, N. (2012). Lateralized auditory cortical alpha band activity and interregional connectivity pattern reflect anticipation of target sounds. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1604–1613. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhr232.PubMed
go back to reference Nee, D. E., & Jonides, J. (2009). Common and distinct neural correlates of perceptual and memorial selection. Neuroimage, 45(3), 963–975.PubMedCentralPubMed Nee, D. E., & Jonides, J. (2009). Common and distinct neural correlates of perceptual and memorial selection. Neuroimage, 45(3), 963–975.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Nobre, A. C., Coull, J. T., Maquet, P., Frith, C. D., Vandenberghe, R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2004). Orienting attention to locations in perceptual versus mental representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 363–373. doi:10.1162/089892904322926700.PubMed Nobre, A. C., Coull, J. T., Maquet, P., Frith, C. D., Vandenberghe, R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2004). Orienting attention to locations in perceptual versus mental representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 363–373. doi:10.​1162/​0898929043229267​00.PubMed
go back to reference O’Craven, K. M., Downing, P. E., & Kanwisher, N. (1999). fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature, 401(6753), 584–587.PubMed O’Craven, K. M., Downing, P. E., & Kanwisher, N. (1999). fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature, 401(6753), 584–587.PubMed
go back to reference Oh, H., & Leung, H. C. (2010). Specific and nonspecific neural activity during selective processing of visual representations in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(2), 292–306. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21250.PubMed Oh, H., & Leung, H. C. (2010). Specific and nonspecific neural activity during selective processing of visual representations in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(2), 292–306. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2009.​21250.PubMed
go back to reference Olsen, R. K., Rondina Ii, R., Riggs, L., Meltzer, J. A., & Ryan, J. D. (2013). Hippocampal and Neocortical Oscillatory Contributions to Visuospatial Binding and Comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,. doi:10.1037/a0034043. Olsen, R. K., Rondina Ii, R., Riggs, L., Meltzer, J. A., & Ryan, J. D. (2013). Hippocampal and Neocortical Oscillatory Contributions to Visuospatial Binding and Comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,. doi:10.​1037/​a0034043.
go back to reference Pelosi, L., Slade, T., Blumhardt, L. D., & Sharma, V. K. (2000). Working memory dysfunction in major depression: an event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(9), 1531–1543. [pii]: S1388-2457(00)00354-0.PubMed Pelosi, L., Slade, T., Blumhardt, L. D., & Sharma, V. K. (2000). Working memory dysfunction in major depression: an event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(9), 1531–1543. [pii]: S1388-2457(00)00354-0.PubMed
go back to reference Picton, T. W. (2010). Human Auditory Evoked Potentials. San Diego: Plural Publishing. Picton, T. W. (2010). Human Auditory Evoked Potentials. San Diego: Plural Publishing.
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.PubMed Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.PubMed
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(2), 160–174.PubMed Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(2), 160–174.PubMed
go back to reference Rama, P., Poremba, A., Sala, J. B., Yee, L., Malloy, M., Mishkin, M., et al. (2004). Dissociable functional cortical topographies for working memory maintenance of voice identity and location. Cerebral Cortex, 14(7), 768–780.PubMed Rama, P., Poremba, A., Sala, J. B., Yee, L., Malloy, M., Mishkin, M., et al. (2004). Dissociable functional cortical topographies for working memory maintenance of voice identity and location. Cerebral Cortex, 14(7), 768–780.PubMed
go back to reference Rauschecker, J. P., & Tian, B. (2000). Mechanisms and streams for processing of “what” and “where” in auditory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11800–11806.PubMedCentralPubMed Rauschecker, J. P., & Tian, B. (2000). Mechanisms and streams for processing of “what” and “where” in auditory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11800–11806.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Reinecke, A., Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2008). How preferential is the preferential encoding of threatening stimuli? Working memory biases in specific anxiety and the Attentional Blink. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(4), 655–670. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.06.004.PubMed Reinecke, A., Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2008). How preferential is the preferential encoding of threatening stimuli? Working memory biases in specific anxiety and the Attentional Blink. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(4), 655–670. doi:10.​1016/​j.​janxdis.​2007.​06.​004.PubMed
go back to reference Rihs, T. A., Michel, C. M., & Thut, G. (2009). A bias for posterior alpha-band power suppression versus enhancement during shifting versus maintenance of spatial attention. Neuroimage, 44(1), 190–199. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.022. Rihs, T. A., Michel, C. M., & Thut, G. (2009). A bias for posterior alpha-band power suppression versus enhancement during shifting versus maintenance of spatial attention. Neuroimage, 44(1), 190–199. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuroimage.​2008.​08.​022.
go back to reference Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rauschecker, J. P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(12), 1131–1136.PubMedCentralPubMed Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rauschecker, J. P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(12), 1131–1136.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Rosler, A., Mapstone, M. E., Hays, A. K., Mesulam, M. M., Rademaker, A., Gitelman, D. R., et al. (2000). Alterations of visual search strategy in Alzheimer’s disease and aging. Neuropsychology, 14(3), 398–408.PubMed Rosler, A., Mapstone, M. E., Hays, A. K., Mesulam, M. M., Rademaker, A., Gitelman, D. R., et al. (2000). Alterations of visual search strategy in Alzheimer’s disease and aging. Neuropsychology, 14(3), 398–408.PubMed
go back to reference Rosler, A., Mapstone, M., Hays-Wicklund, A., Gitelman, D. R., & Weintraub, S. (2005). The “zoom lens” of focal attention in visual search: changes in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 41(4), 512–519.PubMed Rosler, A., Mapstone, M., Hays-Wicklund, A., Gitelman, D. R., & Weintraub, S. (2005). The “zoom lens” of focal attention in visual search: changes in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 41(4), 512–519.PubMed
go back to reference Salmi, J., Rinne, T., Degerman, A., Salonen, O., & Alho, K. (2007). Orienting and maintenance of spatial attention in audition and vision: multimodal and modality-specific brain activations. Brain Structure and Function, 212(2), 181–194.PubMed Salmi, J., Rinne, T., Degerman, A., Salonen, O., & Alho, K. (2007). Orienting and maintenance of spatial attention in audition and vision: multimodal and modality-specific brain activations. Brain Structure and Function, 212(2), 181–194.PubMed
go back to reference Salmi, J., Rinne, T., Koistinen, S., Salonen, O., & Alho, K. (2009). Brain networks of bottom-up triggered and top-down controlled shifting of auditory attention. Brain Research, 1286, 155–164.PubMed Salmi, J., Rinne, T., Koistinen, S., Salonen, O., & Alho, K. (2009). Brain networks of bottom-up triggered and top-down controlled shifting of auditory attention. Brain Research, 1286, 155–164.PubMed
go back to reference Sanders, L. D., & Astheimer, L. B. (2008). Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: event-related potential evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(4), 732–742. Sanders, L. D., & Astheimer, L. B. (2008). Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: event-related potential evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(4), 732–742.
go back to reference Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pecherstorfer, T., Freunberger, R., & Hanslmayr, S. (2005). EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top-down processing in a working memory task. Human Brain Mapping, 26(2), 148–155. doi:10.1002/hbm.20150. Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pecherstorfer, T., Freunberger, R., & Hanslmayr, S. (2005). EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top-down processing in a working memory task. Human Brain Mapping, 26(2), 148–155. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​20150.
go back to reference Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Heise, K. F., Gruber, W. R., Holz, E., Karim, A. A., et al. (2009). Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity. Current Biology: CB, 19(21), 1846–1852. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.062.PubMed Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Heise, K. F., Gruber, W. R., Holz, E., Karim, A. A., et al. (2009). Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity. Current Biology: CB, 19(21), 1846–1852. doi:10.​1016/​j.​cub.​2009.​08.​062.PubMed
go back to reference Schack, B., & Klimesch, W. (2002). Frequency characteristics of evoked and oscillatory electroencephalic activity in a human memory scanning task. Neuroscience Letters, 331(2), 107–110.PubMed Schack, B., & Klimesch, W. (2002). Frequency characteristics of evoked and oscillatory electroencephalic activity in a human memory scanning task. Neuroscience Letters, 331(2), 107–110.PubMed
go back to reference Serences, J. T., Shomstein, S., Leber, A. B., Golay, X., Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (2005). Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychological Science, 16(2), 114–122.PubMed Serences, J. T., Shomstein, S., Leber, A. B., Golay, X., Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (2005). Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychological Science, 16(2), 114–122.PubMed
go back to reference Shahin, A. J., & Pitt, M. A. (2012). Alpha activity marking word boundaries mediates speech segmentation. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 36(12), 3740–3748. doi:10.1111/ejn.12008.PubMed Shahin, A. J., & Pitt, M. A. (2012). Alpha activity marking word boundaries mediates speech segmentation. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 36(12), 3740–3748. doi:10.​1111/​ejn.​12008.PubMed
go back to reference Shen, D., & Alain, C. (2011). Temporal attention facilitates short-term consolidation during a rapid serial auditory presentation task. Experimental Brain Research, 215(3–4), 285–292. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2897-3.PubMed Shen, D., & Alain, C. (2011). Temporal attention facilitates short-term consolidation during a rapid serial auditory presentation task. Experimental Brain Research, 215(3–4), 285–292. doi:10.​1007/​s00221-011-2897-3.PubMed
go back to reference Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2008). Object-based auditory and visual attention. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12(5), 182–186. Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2008). Object-based auditory and visual attention. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12(5), 182–186.
go back to reference Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2002). Object-based attention: sensory modulation or priority setting? Perception & Psychophysics, 64(1), 41–51. Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2002). Object-based attention: sensory modulation or priority setting? Perception & Psychophysics, 64(1), 41–51.
go back to reference Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2004). Control of attention shifts between vision and audition in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(47), 10702–10706.PubMed Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2004). Control of attention shifts between vision and audition in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(47), 10702–10706.PubMed
go back to reference Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2006). Parietal cortex mediates voluntary control of spatial and nonspatial auditory attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(2), 435–439.PubMed Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2006). Parietal cortex mediates voluntary control of spatial and nonspatial auditory attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(2), 435–439.PubMed
go back to reference Shulman, G. L., Ollinger, J. M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Petersen, S. E., et al. (1999). Areas involved in encoding and applying directional expectations to moving objects. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(21), 9480–9496. Shulman, G. L., Ollinger, J. M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Petersen, S. E., et al. (1999). Areas involved in encoding and applying directional expectations to moving objects. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(21), 9480–9496.
go back to reference Smith, K. R., Hsieh, I. H., Saberi, K., & Hickok, G. (2010). Auditory spatial and object processing in the human planum temporale: no evidence for selectivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 632–639. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21196.PubMed Smith, K. R., Hsieh, I. H., Saberi, K., & Hickok, G. (2010). Auditory spatial and object processing in the human planum temporale: no evidence for selectivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 632–639. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2009.​21196.PubMed
go back to reference Snyder, J. S., Alain, C., & Picton, T. W. (2006). Effects of attention on neuroelectric correlates of auditory stream segregation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(1), 1–13.PubMed Snyder, J. S., Alain, C., & Picton, T. W. (2006). Effects of attention on neuroelectric correlates of auditory stream segregation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(1), 1–13.PubMed
go back to reference Sokolov, E. N., & Nezlina, N. I. (2004). Long-term memory, neurogenesis, and signal novelty. [Review]. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 34(8), 847–857.PubMed Sokolov, E. N., & Nezlina, N. I. (2004). Long-term memory, neurogenesis, and signal novelty. [Review]. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 34(8), 847–857.PubMed
go back to reference Spieth, W., Curtis, J. F., & Webster, J. C. (1954). Responding to one of two simultaneous messages. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 26, 391–396. Spieth, W., Curtis, J. F., & Webster, J. C. (1954). Responding to one of two simultaneous messages. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 26, 391–396.
go back to reference Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Peronnet, F., & Pernier, J. (1998). Induced gamma-band activity during the delay of a visual short-term memory task in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 18(11), 4244–4254. Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Peronnet, F., & Pernier, J. (1998). Induced gamma-band activity during the delay of a visual short-term memory task in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 18(11), 4244–4254.
go back to reference Tesche, C. D., & Karhu, J. (2000). Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(2), 919–924.PubMedCentralPubMed Tesche, C. D., & Karhu, J. (2000). Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(2), 919–924.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Treisman, A. M. (1964). The effect of irrelevant material on the efficiency of selective listening. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 533–546.PubMed Treisman, A. M. (1964). The effect of irrelevant material on the efficiency of selective listening. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 533–546.PubMed
go back to reference Tuladhar, A. M., ter Huurne, N., Schoffelen, J. M., Maris, E., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2007). Parieto-occipital sources account for the increase in alpha activity with working memory load. Human Brain Mapping, 28(8), 785–792. doi:10.1002/hbm.20306.PubMed Tuladhar, A. M., ter Huurne, N., Schoffelen, J. M., Maris, E., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2007). Parieto-occipital sources account for the increase in alpha activity with working memory load. Human Brain Mapping, 28(8), 785–792. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​20306.PubMed
go back to reference Ungerleider, L. G., Galkin, T. W., & Mishkin, M. (1983). Visuotopic organization of projections from striate cortex to inferior and lateral pulvinar in rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 217(2), 137–157. doi:10.1002/cne.902170203.PubMed Ungerleider, L. G., Galkin, T. W., & Mishkin, M. (1983). Visuotopic organization of projections from striate cortex to inferior and lateral pulvinar in rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 217(2), 137–157. doi:10.​1002/​cne.​902170203.PubMed
go back to reference Valdes-Sosa, M., Bobes, M. A., Rodriguez, V., & Pinilla, T. (1998). Switching attention without shifting the spotlight object-based attentional modulation of brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(1), 137–151.PubMed Valdes-Sosa, M., Bobes, M. A., Rodriguez, V., & Pinilla, T. (1998). Switching attention without shifting the spotlight object-based attentional modulation of brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(1), 137–151.PubMed
go back to reference Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature, 428(6984), 748–751. doi:10.1038/nature02447. Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature, 428(6984), 748–751. doi:10.​1038/​nature02447.
go back to reference Woldorff, M. G., Gallen, C. C., Hampson, S. A., Hillyard, S. A., Pantev, C., Sobel, D., et al. (1993). Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90(18), 8722–8726.PubMedCentralPubMed Woldorff, M. G., Gallen, C. C., Hampson, S. A., Hillyard, S. A., Pantev, C., Sobel, D., et al. (1993). Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90(18), 8722–8726.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Woods, D. L., & Alain, C. (2001). Conjoining three auditory features: an event-related brain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(4), 492–509.PubMed Woods, D. L., & Alain, C. (2001). Conjoining three auditory features: an event-related brain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(4), 492–509.PubMed
go back to reference Woods, D. L., Alho, K., & Algazi, A. (1991). Brain potential signs of feature processing during auditory selective attention. NeuroReport, 2(4), 189–192.PubMed Woods, D. L., Alho, K., & Algazi, A. (1991). Brain potential signs of feature processing during auditory selective attention. NeuroReport, 2(4), 189–192.PubMed
go back to reference Woods, D. L., Alho, K., & Algazi, A. (1994). Stages of auditory feature conjunction: an event-related brain potential study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(1), 81–94.PubMed Woods, D. L., Alho, K., & Algazi, A. (1994). Stages of auditory feature conjunction: an event-related brain potential study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(1), 81–94.PubMed
go back to reference Worden, M. S., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., & Simpson, G. V. (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 20(6), RC63. Worden, M. S., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., & Simpson, G. V. (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 20(6), RC63.
go back to reference Wu, C. T., Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., & Woldorff, M. G. (2007). The neural circuitry underlying the executive control of auditory spatial attention. Brain Research, 1134(1), 187–198.PubMedCentralPubMed Wu, C. T., Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., & Woldorff, M. G. (2007). The neural circuitry underlying the executive control of auditory spatial attention. Brain Research, 1134(1), 187–198.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Serences, J. T. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 187–193.PubMed Yantis, S., & Serences, J. T. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 187–193.PubMed
go back to reference Yuval-Greenberg, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2007). What you see is not (always) what you hear: induced gamma band responses reflect cross-modal interactions in familiar object recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(5), 1090–1096.PubMed Yuval-Greenberg, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2007). What you see is not (always) what you hear: induced gamma band responses reflect cross-modal interactions in familiar object recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(5), 1090–1096.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Attention to memory: orienting attention to sound object representations
Auteurs
Kristina C. Backer
Claude Alain
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0531-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2014

Psychological Research 3/2014 Naar de uitgave