Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2004

01-02-2004 | Original Article

Assembling a task space: global determination of local shift costs

Auteurs: Thomas Kleinsorge, Herbert Heuer, Volker Schmidtke

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Most theorizing on the origin of the costs that are associated with task shifts focuses on local transitions between individual trials. In the present article we argue that this emphasis has resulted in a neglect of more global representational structures, which also determinate shift costs. To substantiate this claim, we employed a set of four tasks that results from a factorial combination of two types of judgment and two judgment-to-response mappings. From previous work it is known that this kind of task combination is associated with a characteristic profile of shift costs as a function of the relation between successive tasks. Previously we have interpreted this profile as an indication of a hierarchically ordered dimensional representation of the two types of judgment and the two judgment-to-response mappings. Such a representation can only be expected to emerge when the two task features, judgment and mapping, vary independently of each other within the same situation. It is shown that the characteristic shift cost profile can indeed only be observed when the performance of any of the four tasks is required in a block of trials. In contrast, with only two tasks occurring in each block of trials, shift costs do not reflect the relation between successive tasks. This finding confirms the importance of global representational structures as a determinant of shift costs beyond local transitions between individual trials.
Voetnoten
1
We thank Katherine Arbuthnott for directing our attention to the importance of this point.
 
2
An analysis including the response-sequence factor (repetition vs. shift) revealed the typical reversal of response-repetition benefits observed with task repetitions (79 ms) into response-repetition costs in trials with a task shift (33 ms). When we applied the model proposed by Kleinsorge et al. (2002) to the data of Phase 3, the R2 was .987.
 
3
Of course, this does not deny the role of instance-based representations of individual trials, which seem to influence performance, especially after large amounts of practice (e.g., Logan, 1988). However, in all the experiments we have conducted so far with the paradigm of the present study we have never observed a disappearance of the characteristic profile of shift costs that we interpret as indicating generalizing switches as a function of practice, although practice varied within the range of several minutes to several hours.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (2000). 'Task-switching', stimulus-response bindings and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (2000). 'Task-switching', stimulus-response bindings and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Allport, A., Styles, E., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting attentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, A., Styles, E., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting attentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–247. Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–247.
go back to reference Fagot, C. (1994). Chronometric investigations of task switching. Ph.D. thesis, University of California, San Diego. Fagot, C. (1994). Chronometric investigations of task switching. Ph.D. thesis, University of California, San Diego.
go back to reference Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 308–339. Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 308–339.
go back to reference Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching. In: S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Cognitive control. (pp. 331–355). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching. In: S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Cognitive control. (pp. 331–355). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Heuer, H., Schmidtke, V., & Kleinsorge, T. (2001). Implicit learning of sequences of tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 967–983. Heuer, H., Schmidtke, V., & Kleinsorge, T. (2001). Implicit learning of sequences of tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 967–983.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Pösse, B., & Waszak, F. (2000). Contextualization in perception and action. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 227–246. Hommel, B., Pösse, B., & Waszak, F. (2000). Contextualization in perception and action. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 227–246.
go back to reference Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640–653. Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640–653.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T. (2000). Explorations in the structure of task spaces. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 247–258. Kleinsorge, T. (2000). Explorations in the structure of task spaces. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 247–258.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62, 300–312.CrossRef Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62, 300–312.CrossRef
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2001a) Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space is not induced by specific task cues. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 209, 105–117 Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2001a) Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space is not induced by specific task cues. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 209, 105–117
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2001b). Task-set reconfiguration with binary and three-valued task dimensions. Psychological Research, 65, 192–201. Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2001b). Task-set reconfiguration with binary and three-valued task dimensions. Psychological Research, 65, 192–201.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2002). Processes of task-set reconfiguration: Switching operations and implementation operations. Acta Psychologica, 111, 1–28.CrossRefPubMed Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2002). Processes of task-set reconfiguration: Switching operations and implementation operations. Acta Psychologica, 111, 1–28.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (1996). The reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRef Meiran, N. (1996). The reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.CrossRefPubMed Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231. Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.
go back to reference Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic Memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic Memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Assembling a task space: global determination of local shift costs
Auteurs
Thomas Kleinsorge
Herbert Heuer
Volker Schmidtke
Publicatiedatum
01-02-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0134-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2004

Psychological Research 1/2004 Naar de uitgave