Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2012

01-01-2012 | Original Article

Artificial grammar learning in children: abstraction of rules or sensitivity to perceptual features?

Auteurs: Arnaud Witt, Annie Vinter

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We examined sensitivity to grammatical sequences of colors in an artificial grammar learning task in a sample of 120 children aged between 5 and 8 years. The aim of the experiment was to test whether the children would preferentially learn the specific salient features of the items they were exposed to or the rules that generated these items. The children were divided into two experimental groups (identical grammar but training items differing in their surface features) and a control group (random items). The results showed that regardless of age, participants learned the most frequent salient features of the items, as well as some kind of abstract relational information. However, the 8-year-olds presented a more complex result profile, with one of the experimental groups apparently developing sensitivity to grammatical rules. These results are discussed with reference to the main current models of implicit learning. Overall, the results provided more support for stimulus-specific processing models than for rule-based models.
Voetnoten
1
We sincerely thank an anonymous reviewer for this nice metaphor and argument.
 
2
It may be worth explaining why it was not possible to include another test (such as a recognition test, for instance) following the implicit generation test. Recognition is very likely to elicit intentional information recollection processes (Gardiner & Java, 1993), with the result that such a test is a much more explicit test than an implicit generation test. Several authors have pointed out the danger of contamination between implicit and explicit tests when they are jointly administered to the same participants (Gebauer & Mackintosh, 2007; Seger et al., 2000; Shimamura, 1985). Counterbalancing the order between tests does not resolve the problem. A recognition test cannot be administered first because half of the items presented during this test are non-grammatical. However, exposing participants to errors (non-grammatical information) is known to be prejudicial to implicit learning (e.g., Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; Perruchet, et al. 2006) and this can affect performance in a subsequent test. The same reasoning holds if the generation test is presented first. Such a test inevitably leads participants to generate false (non-grammatical) items (partially or completely).
 
3
This does not mean that the children could not evoke the presence of adjacent or non-adjacent repetitions. As we saw in a small number of children, if the experimenter explicitly asked "Were the flags always made of different colors, or were some colors sometimes repeated inside the flags?", they were able to recognize that some colors were sometimes repeated. The explicit knowledge that could be expressed by children in the question phase was highly dependent on the type of questions asked by the experimenter.
 
4
Perruchet and Reber (2003, p. 129) argued that "if trained control subjects do not perform at chance level, this means that some undetected biases are still in operation". The below-chance production of repetitions in the random group probably expressed a natural bias against repetitions. Because the additivity assumption claims that non-specific variables have similar effects on the experimental and control groups trained in very close conditions, if repetition avoidance led to below-chance productions in the control group, it was very likely that sensitivity to repetitive structures raised performance in the experimental groups. However, this increase reached only chance level, not more. For a more direct demonstration of such learning, we computed the ratios between observed and theoretical proportions, and run an ANOVA with Type of series as a between-subjects factor. Type of series yielded significance for the non-adjacent and adjacent repetitions, F(2, 117) = 7.05, p < .01 and F(2, 117) = 18.25, p < .01 respectively. Planned comparisons showed that the non-adjacent group produced more non-adjacent repetitions than the adjacent and random groups, F(1, 117) = 9.46, p < .01 and F(1, 117) = 11.58, p < .01 respectively, as did the adjacent group for the adjacent repetitions, F(1, 117) = 26.85, p < .01 and F(1, 117) = 27.9, p < .01. Thus, there was a specific increase of adjacent or non-adjacent repetitions only in the groups where specific learning was expected.
 
5
The results suggest that the frequent initial bigrams were not learned, as their mean frequency was very close to the theoretical probability. However, it is worth pointing out that the second color in these bigrams was the first color of the repeated adjacent unit (e.g., BYY). Learning the initial bigram (BY) may have been prevented, or at least slowed down, by the formation of the repeated unit (YY). It would appear to be very interesting to study this issue in greater detail because it reveals the limitations of a purely statistical view of learning.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 53–68.PubMedCrossRef Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 53–68.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 169–221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 169–221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Brooks, L. R., & Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: Comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 316–323. Brooks, L. R., & Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: Comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 316–323.
go back to reference Carr, T. H., & Curran, T. (1994). Cognitive factors in learning about structured sequences: Applications to syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 205–230.CrossRef Carr, T. H., & Curran, T. (1994). Cognitive factors in learning about structured sequences: Applications to syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 205–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press. Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Chandler, S. (1993). Are rules and modules really necessary for explaining language? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 593–606. Chandler, S. (1993). Are rules and modules really necessary for explaining language? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 593–606.
go back to reference Endress, A. D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Mehler, J. (2007). Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars. Cognition, 105, 577–614.PubMedCrossRef Endress, A. D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Mehler, J. (2007). Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars. Cognition, 105, 577–614.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.CrossRef Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, U. (1972). Cognitive mechanisms in autism: Experiments with color and tone sequence production. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 160–173.PubMedCrossRef Frith, U. (1972). Cognitive mechanisms in autism: Experiments with color and tone sequence production. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 160–173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallistel, C. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gallistel, C. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Gallistel, C. (2000). The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 1179–1191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gallistel, C. (2000). The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 1179–1191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1993). Recognising and remembering. In A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 163–188). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1993). Recognising and remembering. In A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 163–188). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), 34–54.PubMedCrossRef Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), 34–54.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gomez, R. L., & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70(2), 109–135.PubMedCrossRef Gomez, R. L., & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70(2), 109–135.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jiménez, L. (Ed). (2003). Attention and implicit learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Jiménez, L. (Ed). (2003). Attention and implicit learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
go back to reference Johansson, T. (2009). Strengthening the case for stimulus-specificity in artificial grammar learning. Experimental Psychology, 56, 188–197.PubMedCrossRef Johansson, T. (2009). Strengthening the case for stimulus-specificity in artificial grammar learning. Experimental Psychology, 56, 188–197.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 169–181.PubMedCrossRef Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 169–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krist, H., Fieberg, E. L., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 952–966.CrossRef Krist, H., Fieberg, E. L., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 952–966.CrossRef
go back to reference Ling, J., & Blades, M. (1996). Incidental recall of color information by children and adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 141–150.CrossRef Ling, J., & Blades, M. (1996). Incidental recall of color information by children and adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 141–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Ling, J., & Blades, M. (2002). Further evidence for incidental encoding of color by children and adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 537–544.CrossRef Ling, J., & Blades, M. (2002). Further evidence for incidental encoding of color by children and adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 537–544.CrossRef
go back to reference Manza, L., & Reber, A. S. (1997). Representing artificial grammar: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning (pp. 73–106). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Manza, L., & Reber, A. S. (1997). Representing artificial grammar: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning (pp. 73–106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Mathews, R. C., Buss, R. R., Stanley, W. B., Blanchards-Fields, F., Cho, J. R., & Druhan, B. (1989). Role of implicit and explicit process in learning from examples: A synergistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1083–1100.CrossRef Mathews, R. C., Buss, R. R., Stanley, W. B., Blanchards-Fields, F., Cho, J. R., & Druhan, B. (1989). Role of implicit and explicit process in learning from examples: A synergistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1083–1100.CrossRef
go back to reference Meulemans, T., & Van der Linden, M. (1997). Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1007–1028.CrossRef Meulemans, T., & Van der Linden, M. (1997). Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1007–1028.CrossRef
go back to reference Mittenecker, E. (1958). Analysis of sequences of “chance” action. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 5, 45–60. Mittenecker, E. (1958). Analysis of sequences of “chance” action. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 5, 45–60.
go back to reference Monaghan, P., & Rowson, C. (2008). The effect of repetition and similarity on sequence learning. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1509–1514.CrossRef Monaghan, P., & Rowson, C. (2008). The effect of repetition and similarity on sequence learning. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1509–1514.CrossRef
go back to reference Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning in real world context: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426.CrossRef Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning in real world context: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426.CrossRef
go back to reference Patel, H., Blades, M., & Andrade, J. (1999). Children’s incidental recall of color information. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 41A, 797–828. Patel, H., Blades, M., & Andrade, J. (1999). Children’s incidental recall of color information. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 41A, 797–828.
go back to reference Perruchet, P. (1994). Defining the knowledge units of a synthetic language: Comment on Vokey and Brooks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 223–228.CrossRef Perruchet, P. (1994). Defining the knowledge units of a synthetic language: Comment on Vokey and Brooks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 223–228.CrossRef
go back to reference Perruchet, P., & Reber, R. (2003). Why untrained control groups provide invalid baselines: A reply to Dienes & Altmann. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 125–130. Perruchet, P., & Reber, R. (2003). Why untrained control groups provide invalid baselines: A reply to Dienes & Altmann. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 125–130.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., Rey, A., Hivert, E., & Pacton, S. (2006). Do distractors interfere with memory for study pairs in associative recognition? Memory & Cognition, 34, 1046–1054.CrossRef Perruchet, P., Rey, A., Hivert, E., & Pacton, S. (2006). Do distractors interfere with memory for study pairs in associative recognition? Memory & Cognition, 34, 1046–1054.CrossRef
go back to reference Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (1998). Learning and development. The implicit knowledge assumption reconsidered. In M. Stadler & P. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 495–531). Thousands Oaks: Sage publications. Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (1998). Learning and development. The implicit knowledge assumption reconsidered. In M. Stadler & P. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 495–531). Thousands Oaks: Sage publications.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (2002). The Self-Organizing Consciousness (Target paper). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297–330.PubMed Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (2002). The Self-Organizing Consciousness (Target paper). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297–330.PubMed
go back to reference Picard, D., & Vinter, A. (1999). Representational flexibility in children’s drawings: Effects of age and verbal instructions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 605–622.CrossRef Picard, D., & Vinter, A. (1999). Representational flexibility in children’s drawings: Effects of age and verbal instructions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 605–622.CrossRef
go back to reference Poletiek, F. H., & van Schijndel, T. J. P. (2009). Stimulus set size and statistical coverage of the grammar in artificial grammar learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1058–1064.CrossRef Poletiek, F. H., & van Schijndel, T. J. P. (2009). Stimulus set size and statistical coverage of the grammar in artificial grammar learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1058–1064.CrossRef
go back to reference Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 227–244.PubMedCrossRef Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 227–244.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.CrossRef Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.CrossRef
go back to reference Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235.CrossRef Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235.CrossRef
go back to reference Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press. Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Reber, A. S., & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5, 333–361.CrossRef Reber, A. S., & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5, 333–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Redington, M., & Chater, N. (2002). Knowledge representation and transfer in artificial grammar learning. In R. A. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning. Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press. Redington, M., & Chater, N. (2002). Knowledge representation and transfer in artificial grammar learning. In R. A. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning. Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Seger, C. A., Prabhakaran, V., Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Neural activity differs between explicit and implicit learning of artificial grammar strings: An fMRI study. Psychology, 28, 283–292. Seger, C. A., Prabhakaran, V., Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Neural activity differs between explicit and implicit learning of artificial grammar strings: An fMRI study. Psychology, 28, 283–292.
go back to reference Shimamura, A. P. (1985). Problems with the finding of stochastic independence as evidence for multiple memory systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(1), 1–13.PubMedCrossRef Shimamura, A. P. (1985). Problems with the finding of stochastic independence as evidence for multiple memory systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(1), 1–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, E. E., Langston, C., & Nisbett, R. E. (1992). The case of rules in reasoning. Cognitive science, 16(1), 1–40.CrossRef Smith, E. E., Langston, C., & Nisbett, R. E. (1992). The case of rules in reasoning. Cognitive science, 16(1), 1–40.CrossRef
go back to reference Stadler, M., & Frensch, P. (Eds.) (1998). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousands Oaks: Sage Publications. Stadler, M., & Frensch, P. (Eds.) (1998). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousands Oaks: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Tunney, R. J., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2001). Two modes of transfer in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 614–639.PubMedCrossRef Tunney, R. J., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2001). Two modes of transfer in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 614–639.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.PubMedCrossRef Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vinter, A., & Marot, V. (2007). The development of context sensitivity in children’s graphic copying strategies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 94–110.PubMedCrossRef Vinter, A., & Marot, V. (2007). The development of context sensitivity in children’s graphic copying strategies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 94–110.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (1999). Isolating unconscious influences: The neutral parameter procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 857–875.CrossRef Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (1999). Isolating unconscious influences: The neutral parameter procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 857–875.CrossRef
go back to reference Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 1223–1240.PubMedCrossRef Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 1223–1240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2002). Implicit motor learning through observational training in adults and children. Memory & Cognition, 30, 256–261.CrossRef Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2002). Implicit motor learning through observational training in adults and children. Memory & Cognition, 30, 256–261.CrossRef
go back to reference Vokey, J. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1992). Salience of item knowledge in learning artificial grammars. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 328–344.CrossRef Vokey, J. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1992). Salience of item knowledge in learning artificial grammars. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 328–344.CrossRef
go back to reference Wiegersma, S. (1982). A control theory of sequential response production. Psychological Research, 44, 175–188.CrossRef Wiegersma, S. (1982). A control theory of sequential response production. Psychological Research, 44, 175–188.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Artificial grammar learning in children: abstraction of rules or sensitivity to perceptual features?
Auteurs
Arnaud Witt
Annie Vinter
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0328-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2012

Psychological Research 1/2012 Naar de uitgave