Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies 5/2012

01-10-2012 | Original Paper

Anger and Children’s Socioemotional Development: Can Parenting Elicit a Positive Side to a Negative Emotion?

Auteurs: Rachel A. Razza, Anne Martin, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies | Uitgave 5/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study examined the role of anger in infancy and its interaction with maternal warmth in predicting children’s socioemotional development. Participants included a demographically diverse sample of 316 mothers and children from the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN) study. Infants were followed across 3 waves of data collection from birth through 5 years of age. Mothers reported on infant anger when children were approximately 4 months of age. Maternal warmth was assessed via observation at both 4 months and 2 years. Children’s socioemotional outcomes were assessed at age 5, and included a direct assessment of delay of gratification and maternal reports of internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Anger in infancy significantly predicted higher levels of internalizing and externalizing behaviors at age 5. A main effect of anger on delay of gratification was not supported. However, anger in infancy moderated the association between maternal warmth and delay of gratification, such that only high-anger infants benefited from high maternal warmth. Similar interactive effects were not supported for problem behaviors. These results provide modest support for the differential susceptibility hypothesis, which proposes that highly reactive children are more susceptible to environmental risks and assets than other children. Specifically, findings suggest that although anger can increase children’s vulnerability to problem behaviors, it can also be a motivating factor for self-regulation in the presence of supportive parenting.
Literatuur
go back to reference Achenbach, T. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. Achenbach, T. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.
go back to reference Allison, P. D. (2009). Missing data. In R. E. Millsap & A. Maydeu-Olivares (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative methods in psychology (pp. 72–89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Allison, P. D. (2009). Missing data. In R. E. Millsap & A. Maydeu-Olivares (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative methods in psychology (pp. 72–89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edn). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edn). Washington, DC: Author.
go back to reference Bates, J. E. (1989). Applications of temperament concepts. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 3–26). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Bates, J. E. (1989). Applications of temperament concepts. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 3–26). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
go back to reference Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186.CrossRef Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186.CrossRef
go back to reference Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300–304.CrossRef Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300–304.CrossRef
go back to reference Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301–319.PubMedCrossRef Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301–319.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berkowitz, L., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion, 4, 107–130.PubMedCrossRef Berkowitz, L., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion, 4, 107–130.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2010). Contributions of child’s physiology and maternal behavior to children’s trajectories of temperamental reactivity. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1089–1102.PubMedCrossRef Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2010). Contributions of child’s physiology and maternal behavior to children’s trajectories of temperamental reactivity. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1089–1102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 791–804.PubMedCrossRef Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 791–804.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brooks-Gunn, J., & Markman, L. (2005). The contribution of parenting to ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness. The Future of Children, 15, 138–167.CrossRef Brooks-Gunn, J., & Markman, L. (2005). The contribution of parenting to ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness. The Future of Children, 15, 138–167.CrossRef
go back to reference Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359–374.PubMed Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359–374.PubMed
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., Amaral, D. G., Blanchard, J. J., Cameron, J. L., Carter, C. S., Crews, D., et al. (2007). Social neuroscience: Progress and implications for mental health. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 99–123.CrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., Amaral, D. G., Blanchard, J. J., Cameron, J. L., Carter, C. S., Crews, D., et al. (2007). Social neuroscience: Progress and implications for mental health. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 99–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)—Revised Edition. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas. Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)—Revised Edition. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas.
go back to reference Calkins, S. D. (2002). Does aversive behavior during toddlerhood matter? The effects of difficult temperament on maternal perceptions and behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 381–402.CrossRef Calkins, S. D. (2002). Does aversive behavior during toddlerhood matter? The effects of difficult temperament on maternal perceptions and behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 381–402.CrossRef
go back to reference Campos, J., Campos, R., & Barrett, K. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394–402.CrossRef Campos, J., Campos, R., & Barrett, K. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394–402.CrossRef
go back to reference Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183–204.PubMedCrossRef Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183–204.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology doi:10.1037/a0021316. Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology doi:10.​1037/​a0021316.
go back to reference Denham, S. A., Lehman, E. B., Moser, M. H., & Reeves, S. L. (1995). Continuity and change in emotional components of infant temperament. Child Study Journal, 25, 289–308. Denham, S. A., Lehman, E. B., Moser, M. H., & Reeves, S. L. (1995). Continuity and change in emotional components of infant temperament. Child Study Journal, 25, 289–308.
go back to reference Dennis, T. (2006). Emotional self-regulation in preschoolers: The interplay of child approach reactivity, parenting, and control capacities. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 84–97.PubMedCrossRef Dennis, T. (2006). Emotional self-regulation in preschoolers: The interplay of child approach reactivity, parenting, and control capacities. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 84–97.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652.PubMedCrossRef Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Early temperament and emotional development. In A. F. Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Handbook of brain and behaviour in human development (pp. 967–988). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Early temperament and emotional development. In A. F. Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Handbook of brain and behaviour in human development (pp. 967–988). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, A., Reiser, M., et al. (2003). Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity, children’s regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 3–19.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, A., Reiser, M., et al. (2003). Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity, children’s regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 3–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., Reiser, M., et al. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 988–1008.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., Reiser, M., et al. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 988–1008.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76, 1055–1071.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76, 1055–1071.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fox, N. A. (1994). Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Monographs of the society for research in child development: Vol. 59. The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations (pp. 152–166). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Fox, N. A. (1994). Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Monographs of the society for research in child development: Vol. 59. The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations (pp. 152–166). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Fuligni, A. S., Han, W.-J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). The infant-toddler HOME in the second and third years of life. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 139–159. Fuligni, A. S., Han, W.-J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). The infant-toddler HOME in the second and third years of life. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 139–159.
go back to reference Gaertner, B., Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Attention in toddlers: Measurement, stability, and relations to negative emotion and parenting. Infant and Child Development, 17, 339–363.PubMedCrossRef Gaertner, B., Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Attention in toddlers: Measurement, stability, and relations to negative emotion and parenting. Infant and Child Development, 17, 339–363.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 313–333.PubMedCrossRef Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 313–333.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. London, England: Oxford University Press. Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. London, England: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Graziano, P. A., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Maternal behaviour and children’s early emotion regulation skills differentially predict development of children’s reactive control and later effortful control. Infant and Child Development, 19, 333–353.PubMed Graziano, P. A., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Maternal behaviour and children’s early emotion regulation skills differentially predict development of children’s reactive control and later effortful control. Infant and Child Development, 19, 333–353.PubMed
go back to reference Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and prefrontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310–1316.PubMedCrossRef Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and prefrontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310–1316.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henderson, H. A., & Wachs, T. D. (2007). Temperament theory and the study of cognition-emotion interactions across development. Developmental Review, 27, 396–427.CrossRef Henderson, H. A., & Wachs, T. D. (2007). Temperament theory and the study of cognition-emotion interactions across development. Developmental Review, 27, 396–427.CrossRef
go back to reference Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and family socialization strategies and children’s mental health: Low-income, Mexican-American, and Euro-American mothers and children. Child Development, 74, 189–204.PubMedCrossRef Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and family socialization strategies and children’s mental health: Low-income, Mexican-American, and Euro-American mothers and children. Child Development, 74, 189–204.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development: I. Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. In J. D. Ososfsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd edn ed., pp. 494–554). Oxford, England: Wiley. Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development: I. Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. In J. D. Ososfsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd edn ed., pp. 494–554). Oxford, England: Wiley.
go back to reference Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., Feng, W. Y., Hendler, J., & Greene, S. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national comparison. Developmental Psychology, 30, 342–345.CrossRef Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., Feng, W. Y., Hendler, J., & Greene, S. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national comparison. Developmental Psychology, 30, 342–345.CrossRef
go back to reference Kessler, R. C., & Mrozek, D. (1997). UM CIDI-short form. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research. Kessler, R. C., & Mrozek, D. (1997). UM CIDI-short form. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research.
go back to reference Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 325–347.CrossRef Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 325–347.CrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mother’s discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597–615.CrossRef Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mother’s discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597–615.CrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple ways to conscience for children with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple ways to conscience for children with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 339–351.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 339–351.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.PubMedCrossRef Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kopp, C. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.CrossRef Kopp, C. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.CrossRef
go back to reference Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Keenan, K., Rathouz, P. J., D’Onofrio, B. M., Rodgers, J. L., et al. (2008). Temperament and parenting during the first year of life predict future child conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1139–1158.PubMedCrossRef Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Keenan, K., Rathouz, P. J., D’Onofrio, B. M., Rodgers, J. L., et al. (2008). Temperament and parenting during the first year of life predict future child conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1139–1158.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laptook, R. S., Klein, D. N., Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., Olino, T. M., & Carlson, G. (2008). Differentiation between los positive affectivity and behavioral inhibition in preschool age children: A comparison of behavioral approach in novel and non-novel contexts. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 758–767.PubMedCrossRef Laptook, R. S., Klein, D. N., Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., Olino, T. M., & Carlson, G. (2008). Differentiation between los positive affectivity and behavioral inhibition in preschool age children: A comparison of behavioral approach in novel and non-novel contexts. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 758–767.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lawson, K. R., & Ruff, H. A. (2004). Early attention and negative emotionality predict later cognitive and behavioural function. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 157–165.CrossRef Lawson, K. R., & Ruff, H. A. (2004). Early attention and negative emotionality predict later cognitive and behavioural function. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 157–165.CrossRef
go back to reference Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762–775.PubMedCrossRef Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762–775.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leventhal, T., Martin, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). The Early Childhood – Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (EC-HOME): Across five national datasets in the third and fifth year of life. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 161–188. Leventhal, T., Martin, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). The Early Childhood – Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (EC-HOME): Across five national datasets in the third and fifth year of life. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 161–188.
go back to reference Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745–751.CrossRef Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745–751.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Ramsey, D. S., & Alessandri, S. M. (1992). Individual differences in anger and sad expressions during extinction: Antecedents and consequences. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 443–452.CrossRef Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Ramsey, D. S., & Alessandri, S. M. (1992). Individual differences in anger and sad expressions during extinction: Antecedents and consequences. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 443–452.CrossRef
go back to reference Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Heatherington & P. H. Mussen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4: Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley. Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Heatherington & P. H. Mussen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4: Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference McEwen, B. S. (2005). Stressed or stressed out: What is the difference? Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30, 315–318.PubMed McEwen, B. S. (2005). Stressed or stressed out: What is the difference? Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30, 315–318.PubMed
go back to reference Mikulincer, M. (1988). Reactance and helplessness following exposure to unsolvable problems: The effects of attributional style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 679–686.PubMedCrossRef Mikulincer, M. (1988). Reactance and helplessness following exposure to unsolvable problems: The effects of attributional style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 679–686.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology, 44, 771–786.PubMedCrossRef Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology, 44, 771–786.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morrell, J., & Murray, L. (2003). Parenting and the development of conduct disorder and hyperactive symptoms in childhood: A prospective longitudinal study from 2 months to 8 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 489–508.PubMedCrossRef Morrell, J., & Murray, L. (2003). Parenting and the development of conduct disorder and hyperactive symptoms in childhood: A prospective longitudinal study from 2 months to 8 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 489–508.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., & Essex, M. J. (2002). Temperament vulnerability and negative parenting as interacting predictors of child adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 461–471.CrossRef Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., & Essex, M. J. (2002). Temperament vulnerability and negative parenting as interacting predictors of child adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 461–471.CrossRef
go back to reference Murphy, B., & Eisenberg, N. (1996). Provoked by a peer: Children’s anger related responses and their relation to social functioning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 103–124. Murphy, B., & Eisenberg, N. (1996). Provoked by a peer: Children’s anger related responses and their relation to social functioning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 103–124.
go back to reference Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach: Vol. 3. Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia. Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach: Vol. 3. Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.
go back to reference Paulussen-Hoogeboom, M. C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Hermanns, J. M. A., Thea, T. D., & van den Wittenboer, G. L. H. (2008). Parenting style as a mediator between children’s negative emotionality and problematic behavior in early childhood. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169, 209–226.PubMedCrossRef Paulussen-Hoogeboom, M. C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Hermanns, J. M. A., Thea, T. D., & van den Wittenboer, G. L. H. (2008). Parenting style as a mediator between children’s negative emotionality and problematic behavior in early childhood. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169, 209–226.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2002). Development of approach and inhibition in the first year: Parallel findings from motor behavior, temperament ratings and directional cardiac response. Developmental Science, 5, 441–451.CrossRef Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2002). Development of approach and inhibition in the first year: Parallel findings from motor behavior, temperament ratings and directional cardiac response. Developmental Science, 5, 441–451.CrossRef
go back to reference Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76, 212–226.PubMedCrossRef Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76, 212–226.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Raver, C. C. (1996). The relations between social contingency in mother-child interactions and 24-month-olds’ social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 850–859.CrossRef Raver, C. C. (1996). The relations between social contingency in mother-child interactions and 24-month-olds’ social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 850–859.CrossRef
go back to reference Rodriguez, M. L., Ayduk, O., Aber, J. L., Mischel, W., Sethi, A., & Shoda, Y. (2005). A contextual approach to the development of self-regulatory competencies: The role of maternal unresponsitivity and toddlers’ negative affect in stressful situations. Social Development, 14, 136–157.CrossRef Rodriguez, M. L., Ayduk, O., Aber, J. L., Mischel, W., Sethi, A., & Shoda, Y. (2005). A contextual approach to the development of self-regulatory competencies: The role of maternal unresponsitivity and toddlers’ negative affect in stressful situations. Social Development, 14, 136–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRef Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRef
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365.CrossRef Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365.CrossRef
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to 7 years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–1408.PubMedCrossRef Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to 7 years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–1408.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th edn, pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th edn, pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at 7 years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span (pp. 85–119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at 7 years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span (pp. 85–119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Executive attention and effortful control: Linking temperament, brain networks, and genes. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 2–7.CrossRef Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Executive attention and effortful control: Linking temperament, brain networks, and genes. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 2–7.CrossRef
go back to reference Royston, P. (2007). Multiple imputation of missing values: Further update of ice, with an emphasis on interval censoring. The Stata Journal, 7, 445–464. Royston, P. (2007). Multiple imputation of missing values: Further update of ice, with an emphasis on interval censoring. The Stata Journal, 7, 445–464.
go back to reference Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918–924.PubMedCrossRef Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918–924.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections between temperament and social development: A review. Social Development, 13, 142–170.CrossRef Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections between temperament and social development: A review. Social Development, 13, 142–170.CrossRef
go back to reference Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., & Vondra, J. I. (1994). Developmental precursors of externalizing behavior: Ages 1 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 30, 335–364.CrossRef Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., & Vondra, J. I. (1994). Developmental precursors of externalizing behavior: Ages 1 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 30, 335–364.CrossRef
go back to reference Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., et al. (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1170–1186.PubMedCrossRef Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., et al. (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1170–1186.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 401–421.PubMedCrossRef Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 401–421.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Stolk, M. N., Alink, L. R. A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., et al. (2007). Differential susceptibility to discipline: The moderating effect of child temperament on the association between maternal discipline and early childhood externalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 626–636.PubMedCrossRef van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Stolk, M. N., Alink, L. R. A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., et al. (2007). Differential susceptibility to discipline: The moderating effect of child temperament on the association between maternal discipline and early childhood externalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 626–636.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference von Hippel, P. T. (2007). Regression with missing Ys: An improved strategy for analyzing multiply imputed data. Sociological Methodology, 37, 83–117.CrossRef von Hippel, P. T. (2007). Regression with missing Ys: An improved strategy for analyzing multiply imputed data. Sociological Methodology, 37, 83–117.CrossRef
go back to reference Zaslow, M. J., Weinfield, N. S., Gallagher, M., Hair, E. C., Ogawa, J. R., Egeland, B., et al. (2006). Longitudinal prediction of child outcomes from differing measures of parenting in a low-income sample. Developmental Psychology, 42, 27–37.PubMedCrossRef Zaslow, M. J., Weinfield, N. S., Gallagher, M., Hair, E. C., Ogawa, J. R., Egeland, B., et al. (2006). Longitudinal prediction of child outcomes from differing measures of parenting in a low-income sample. Developmental Psychology, 42, 27–37.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Anger and Children’s Socioemotional Development: Can Parenting Elicit a Positive Side to a Negative Emotion?
Auteurs
Rachel A. Razza
Anne Martin
Jeanne Brooks-Gunn
Publicatiedatum
01-10-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Child and Family Studies / Uitgave 5/2012
Print ISSN: 1062-1024
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2843
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-011-9545-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2012

Journal of Child and Family Studies 5/2012 Naar de uitgave