Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2020

27-09-2018 | Original Article

An attentional blink in the absence of spatial attention: a cost of awareness?

Auteurs: Eyal Alef Ophir, Eyal Sherman, Dominique Lamy

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The attentional blink refers to the finding that when two visual targets appear within 200–500 ms, observers often miss the second target. In three experiments, we disentangle the roles of spatial attention to and conscious report of the first event in eliciting this cost. We show that allocating spatial attention to the first event is not necessary for a blink to occur: the full temporal pattern of the blink arises when the first event is consciously detected, despite the fact that it is not spatially attended, whereas no cost is observed when the first event is missed. We then show that spatial attention is also not sufficient for eliciting a blink, though it can deepen the blink when accompanied by conscious detection. These results demonstrate that there is no cost associated with the initiation of an attentional episode, whereas explicit conscious detection comes at a price. These findings demonstrate the temporal flexibility of attention and underscore the potential role of subjective awareness in understanding processing limitations, although this role may be contingent on the encoding in working memory necessary for conscious report.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Theeuwes and colleagues have claimed that salient irrelevant color cues mandatorily capture attention (e.g., Theeuwes, Atchley & Kramer, 2000; see Theeuwes, 2010, for a review). However, support for this claim comes almost exclusively from experiments in which the target had a unique feature and could be found by monitoring the displays for a featural discontinuity (i.e., using a search strategy known as the “singleton-detection mode”, Bacon & Egeth, 1994). Evidence for attentional capture by a cue that has a salient feature outside the observer’s attentional state is scarce and has been mostly reported for onset cues (e.g., Folk & Remington, 2015).
 
2
This same-location cost has been reported in several previous studies (e.g., Anderson & Folk, 2012; Becker, Folk, & Remington, 2013, Experiment 3; Belopolsky et al., 2010; Carmel & Lamy, 2014; 2015; Eimer, Kiss, Press, & Sauter, 2009; Folk & Remington, 2008; Lamy et al., 2015; Lamy, Leber & Egeth, 2004; Schönhammer & Kerzel, 2013). Importantly, although the mechanisms underlying this same-location cost are debated, Carmel and Lamy (2014; 2015) demonstrated that this cost is unrelated to attention and is contingent on conscious perception of the cue.
 
3
Note that these findings do not contradict the widely accepted idea that attention is necessary for conscious perception (e.g., Dehaene, Changeux, Naccache, Sackur & Sergent, 2006 but see Tsuchiya & Koch, 2016). Indeed, previous research has shown that a stimulus that must be responded to can be consciously detected even if it benefits only from very little—distributed—spatial attention (e.g., Mack & Rock, 1998; Fei-Fei et al., 2005). In Lamy et al.’s (2015) study, participants were asked to rate the irrelevant-color cue’s visibility—a task that required no more than detecting a color singleton, and therefore, did not require spatial attention. By contrast, relevant-color cues captured attention, yet were sometimes missed. This finding is consistent with previous reports showing that spatial attention is not sufficient for conscious perception (e.g., Kentridge, Nijboer & Heywood, 2008).
 
4
The reason why the cue color in one group did not exactly match the target color in the other group is that different factors constrained the choice of the target and cue colors. On the one hand, the target color had to be discriminable enough for baseline performance to remain relatively high, as is characteristic of previous AB studies. On the other hand, the cue color had to be faint enough for participants to be entirely unaware of its presence (and rate its visibility as null) on a sizeable proportion of the trials. Note that the latter constraint differed in this experiment relative to the previous one, in which CFS was used: in Experiment 3, the cue had to be strong enough to overcome suppression on enough trials to elicit above 0 visibility ratings.
 
5
Previous studies reported the incidental finding that RTs to a target are slower when this target follows a prime that is consciously perceived relative to when this prime escapes awareness (e.g., Lamy et al., 2015; Peremen & Lamy, 2014a, b; see also Van den Bussche et al., 2013). As the time interval between the prime and target in these studies typically fell within the range of the blink period, the observed impairment is likely to reflect, at least in part, the same cost of awareness as reported in the present study.
 
6
McKay and Juola (2007) showed that spatial and temporal cues are associated with independent cueing benefits. However, this finding only entails that observers can take advantage of two separate sources of knowledge and that these have additive effects on performance. McKay and Juola’s (2007) finding does not entail that spatial selection and temporal attentional selection per se, operate independently of each other.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Akyürek, E. G., Eshuis, S. A., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Saija, J. D., Başkent, D., & Hommel, B. (2012). Temporal target integration underlies performance at lag 1 in the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1448.PubMed Akyürek, E. G., Eshuis, S. A., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Saija, J. D., Başkent, D., & Hommel, B. (2012). Temporal target integration underlies performance at lag 1 in the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1448.PubMed
go back to reference Akyürek, E. G., & Hommel, B. (2005). Target integration and the attentional blink. Acta Psychologica, 119(3), 305–314.PubMedCrossRef Akyürek, E. G., & Hommel, B. (2005). Target integration and the attentional blink. Acta Psychologica, 119(3), 305–314.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74(6), 1183–1198.CrossRef Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74(6), 1183–1198.CrossRef
go back to reference Andrade, J. (2001). The contribution of working memory to conscious experience. Working Memory in Perspective, 60–78. Andrade, J. (2001). The contribution of working memory to conscious experience. Working Memory in Perspective, 60–78.
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 937.PubMed Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 937.PubMed
go back to reference Baars, B. J. (1997). Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception, and working memory. Consciousness and cognition, 6(2), 363–371.PubMedCrossRef Baars, B. J. (1997). Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception, and working memory. Consciousness and cognition, 6(2), 363–371.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2003). How conscious experience and working memory interact. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(4), 166–172.PubMedCrossRef Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2003). How conscious experience and working memory interact. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(4), 166–172.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception and Psychophysics, 55(5), 485–496.PubMedCrossRef Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception and Psychophysics, 55(5), 485–496.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24(5), 634–647.PubMedCrossRef Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24(5), 634–647.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Belopolsky, A. V., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). What is top-down about contingent capture? Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72(2), 326–341.CrossRef Belopolsky, A. V., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). What is top-down about contingent capture? Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72(2), 326–341.CrossRef
go back to reference Bowman, H., & Wyble, B. (2007). The simultaneous type, serial token model of temporal attention and working memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 38.PubMedCrossRef Bowman, H., & Wyble, B. (2007). The simultaneous type, serial token model of temporal attention and working memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 38.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. (1987). From detection to identification: Response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception and Psychophysics, 42(2), 105–113.PubMedCrossRef Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. (1987). From detection to identification: Response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception and Psychophysics, 42(2), 105–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2014). The same-location cost is unrelated to attentional settings: An object-updating account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1465.PubMed Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2014). The same-location cost is unrelated to attentional settings: An object-updating account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1465.PubMed
go back to reference Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2015). Towards a resolution of the attentional-capture debate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(6), 1772.PubMed Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2015). Towards a resolution of the attentional-capture debate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(6), 1772.PubMed
go back to reference Chen, P., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2007). Contingent capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against rapid disengagement. Visual Cognition, 15(6), 637–646.CrossRef Chen, P., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2007). Contingent capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against rapid disengagement. Visual Cognition, 15(6), 637–646.CrossRef
go back to reference Chua, F. K. (2015). A moving overlay shrinks the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77(1), 173–189.CrossRef Chua, F. K. (2015). A moving overlay shrinks the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77(1), 173–189.CrossRef
go back to reference Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101.CrossRefPubMed Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chun, M. M., & Potter, M. C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 109.PubMed Chun, M. M., & Potter, M. C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 109.PubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211.PubMedCrossRef Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dell’Acqua, R., Pierre, J., Pascali, A., & Pluchino, P. (2007). Short-term consolidation of individual identities leads to Lag-1 sparing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 593.PubMed Dell’Acqua, R., Pierre, J., Pascali, A., & Pluchino, P. (2007). Short-term consolidation of individual identities leads to Lag-1 sparing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 593.PubMed
go back to reference Di Lollo, V., Kawahara, J. I., Ghorashi, S. S., & Enns, J. T. (2005). The attentional blink: Resource depletion or temporary loss of control? Psychological Research, 69(3), 191–200.PubMedCrossRef Di Lollo, V., Kawahara, J. I., Ghorashi, S. S., & Enns, J. T. (2005). The attentional blink: Resource depletion or temporary loss of control? Psychological Research, 69(3), 191–200.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dux, P. E., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2008). An attentional blink for sequentially presented targets: Evidence in favor of resource depletion accounts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15(4), 809–813.PubMedCrossRef Dux, P. E., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2008). An attentional blink for sequentially presented targets: Evidence in favor of resource depletion accounts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15(4), 809–813.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dux, P. E., & Harris, I. M. (2007). On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(4), 723–728.PubMedCrossRef Dux, P. E., & Harris, I. M. (2007). On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(4), 723–728.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dux, P. E., & Marois, R. (2009). The attentional blink: A review of data and theory. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71(8), 1683–1700.CrossRef Dux, P. E., & Marois, R. (2009). The attentional blink: A review of data and theory. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71(8), 1683–1700.CrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., Kiss, M., Press, C., & Sauter, D. (2009). The roles of feature-specific task set and bottom-up salience in attentional capture: an ERP study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1316.PubMed Eimer, M., Kiss, M., Press, C., & Sauter, D. (2009). The roles of feature-specific task set and bottom-up salience in attentional capture: an ERP study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1316.PubMed
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRefPubMed Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fei-Fei, L., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., & Perona, P. (2005). Why does natural scene categorization require little attention? Exploring attentional requirements for natural and synthetic stimuli. Visual Cognition, 12(6), 893–924.CrossRef Fei-Fei, L., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., & Perona, P. (2005). Why does natural scene categorization require little attention? Exploring attentional requirements for natural and synthetic stimuli. Visual Cognition, 12(6), 893–924.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Ester, E. F., & Troemel, K. (2009). How to keep attention from straying: Get engaged! Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(1), 127–132.PubMedCrossRef Folk, C. L., Ester, E. F., & Troemel, K. (2009). How to keep attention from straying: Get engaged! Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(1), 127–132.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2002). Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink. Perception and Psychophysics, 64(5), 741–753.PubMedCrossRef Folk, C. L., Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2002). Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink. Perception and Psychophysics, 64(5), 741–753.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 24(3), 847.PubMed Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 24(3), 847.PubMed
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16(2–3), 215–231.CrossRef Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16(2–3), 215–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1153.PubMed Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1153.PubMed
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030.PubMed Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030.PubMed
go back to reference Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., & Lien, M. C. (2016). The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1104.PubMed Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., & Lien, M. C. (2016). The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1104.PubMed
go back to reference Harris, I. M., Benito, C. T., & Dux, P. E. (2010). Priming from distractors in rapid serial visual presentation is modulated by image properties and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1595.PubMed Harris, I. M., Benito, C. T., & Dux, P. E. (2010). Priming from distractors in rapid serial visual presentation is modulated by image properties and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1595.PubMed
go back to reference Holländer, A., Corballis, M. C., & Hamm, J. P. (2005). Visual-field asymmetry in dual-stream RSVP. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 35–40.PubMedCrossRef Holländer, A., Corballis, M. C., & Hamm, J. P. (2005). Visual-field asymmetry in dual-stream RSVP. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 35–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Akyürek, E. G. (2005). Lag-1 sparing in the attentional blink: Benefits and costs of integrating two events into a single episode. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(8), 1415–1433.CrossRef Hommel, B., & Akyürek, E. G. (2005). Lag-1 sparing in the attentional blink: Benefits and costs of integrating two events into a single episode. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(8), 1415–1433.CrossRef
go back to reference Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434–446.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434–446.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Jeffreys, H. (1998). The theory of probability. OUP, Oxford. Jeffreys, H. (1998). The theory of probability. OUP, Oxford.
go back to reference Jefferies, L. N., Enns, J. T., & Di Lollo, V. (2014). The flexible focus: Whether spatial attention is unitary or divided depends on observer goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 465.PubMed Jefferies, L. N., Enns, J. T., & Di Lollo, V. (2014). The flexible focus: Whether spatial attention is unitary or divided depends on observer goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 465.PubMed
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P. (1999). Concurrent response-selection demands modulate the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 25(4), 1097. Jolicoeur, P. (1999). Concurrent response-selection demands modulate the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 25(4), 1097.
go back to reference Jolicœur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1999). Attentional and structural constraints on visual encoding. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 62(2–3), 154–164. Jolicœur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1999). Attentional and structural constraints on visual encoding. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 62(2–3), 154–164.
go back to reference Kass, R. E., & Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90(430), 773–795.CrossRef Kass, R. E., & Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90(430), 773–795.CrossRef
go back to reference Kawahara, J. I., Kumada, T., & Di Lollo, V. (2006). The attentional blink is governed by a temporary loss of control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(5), 886–890.PubMedCrossRef Kawahara, J. I., Kumada, T., & Di Lollo, V. (2006). The attentional blink is governed by a temporary loss of control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(5), 886–890.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kentridge, R. W., Nijboer, T. C., & Heywood, C. A. (2008). Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 864–869.PubMedCrossRef Kentridge, R. W., Nijboer, T. C., & Heywood, C. A. (2008). Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 864–869.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kiss, M., Van Velzen, J., & Eimer, M. (2008). The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing. Psychophysiology, 45(2), 240–249.PubMedCrossRef Kiss, M., Van Velzen, J., & Eimer, M. (2008). The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing. Psychophysiology, 45(2), 240–249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 16–22.PubMedCrossRef Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 16–22.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D. (2005). Temporal expectations modulate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(6), 1112–1119.PubMedCrossRef Lamy, D. (2005). Temporal expectations modulate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(6), 1112–1119.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lamme, V. A. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in cognitive sciences, 10(11), 494–501.PubMedCrossRef Lamme, V. A. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in cognitive sciences, 10(11), 494–501.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D., Leber, A., & Egeth, H. E. (2004). Effects of task relevance and stimulus-driven salience in feature-search mode. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(6), 1019–1031.PubMed Lamy, D., Leber, A., & Egeth, H. E. (2004). Effects of task relevance and stimulus-driven salience in feature-search mode. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(6), 1019–1031.PubMed
go back to reference Leblanc, É, & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). The time course of the contingent spatial blink. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(2), 124.PubMedCrossRef Leblanc, É, & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). The time course of the contingent spatial blink. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(2), 124.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Livesey, E. J., & Harris, I. M. (2011). Target sparing effects in the attentional blink depend on type of stimulus. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2104–2123.CrossRef Livesey, E. J., & Harris, I. M. (2011). Target sparing effects in the attentional blink depend on type of stimulus. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2104–2123.CrossRef
go back to reference Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness (Vol. 33). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.CrossRef Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness (Vol. 33). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.CrossRef
go back to reference MacKay, A., & Juola, J. F. (2007). Are spatial and temporal attention independent? Perception and Psychophysics, 69(6), 972–979.CrossRef MacKay, A., & Juola, J. F. (2007). Are spatial and temporal attention independent? Perception and Psychophysics, 69(6), 972–979.CrossRef
go back to reference MacLean, M. H., & Arnell, K. M. (2012). A conceptual and methodological framework for measuring and modulating the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74(6), 1080–1097.CrossRef MacLean, M. H., & Arnell, K. M. (2012). A conceptual and methodological framework for measuring and modulating the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74(6), 1080–1097.CrossRef
go back to reference Martens, S., & Wyble, B. (2010). The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 947–957.PubMedCrossRef Martens, S., & Wyble, B. (2010). The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 947–957.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 168.PubMed McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 168.PubMed
go back to reference Most, S. B., Scholl, B. J., Clifford, E. R., & Simons, D. J. (2005). What you see is what you set: sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychological Review, 112(1), 217.PubMedCrossRef Most, S. B., Scholl, B. J., Clifford, E. R., & Simons, D. J. (2005). What you see is what you set: sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychological Review, 112(1), 217.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mulckhuyse, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 299–309.PubMedCrossRef Mulckhuyse, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 299–309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nieuwenhuis, S., Gilzenrat, M. S., Holmes, B. D., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). The role of the locus coeruleus in mediating the attentional blink: a neurocomputational theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 291.CrossRef Nieuwenhuis, S., Gilzenrat, M. S., Holmes, B. D., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). The role of the locus coeruleus in mediating the attentional blink: a neurocomputational theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 291.CrossRef
go back to reference Nieuwenstein, M., Van der Burg, E., Theeuwes, J., Wyble, B., & Potter, M. (2009). Temporal constraints on conscious vision: On the ubiquitous nature of the attentional blink. Journal of Vision, 9(9), 18–18.PubMedCrossRef Nieuwenstein, M., Van der Burg, E., Theeuwes, J., Wyble, B., & Potter, M. (2009). Temporal constraints on conscious vision: On the ubiquitous nature of the attentional blink. Journal of Vision, 9(9), 18–18.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nieuwenstein, M. R., & Potter, M. C. (2006). Temporal limits of selection and memory encoding a comparison of whole versus partial report in rapid serial visual presentation. Psychological Science, 17(6), 471–475.PubMedCrossRef Nieuwenstein, M. R., & Potter, M. C. (2006). Temporal limits of selection and memory encoding a comparison of whole versus partial report in rapid serial visual presentation. Psychological Science, 17(6), 471–475.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olivers, C. N., & Meeter, M. (2008). A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. Psychological review, 115(4), 836.PubMedCrossRef Olivers, C. N., & Meeter, M. (2008). A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. Psychological review, 115(4), 836.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olivers, C. N., Van Der Stigchel, S., & Hulleman, J. (2007). Spreading the sparing: Against a limited-capacity account of the attentional blink. Psychological Research, 71(2), 126–139.PubMedCrossRef Olivers, C. N., Van Der Stigchel, S., & Hulleman, J. (2007). Spreading the sparing: Against a limited-capacity account of the attentional blink. Psychological Research, 71(2), 126–139.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oriet, C., Pandey, M., & Kawahara, J. I. (2017). Attention capture without awareness in a non-spatial selection task. Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 117–128.PubMedCrossRef Oriet, C., Pandey, M., & Kawahara, J. I. (2017). Attention capture without awareness in a non-spatial selection task. Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 117–128.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Peremen, Z., & Lamy, D. (2014). Do conscious perception and unconscious processing rely on independent mechanisms? A meta-contrast study. Consciousness and Cognition, 24, 22–32.PubMedCrossRef Peremen, Z., & Lamy, D. (2014). Do conscious perception and unconscious processing rely on independent mechanisms? A meta-contrast study. Consciousness and Cognition, 24, 22–32.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Peremen, Z., & Lamy, D. (2015). Comparing unconscious processing during continuous flash suppression and meta-contrast masking just under the limen of consciousness. Invisible, but how? The depth of unconscious processing as inferred from different suppression techniques. 109. Peremen, Z., & Lamy, D. (2015). Comparing unconscious processing during continuous flash suppression and meta-contrast masking just under the limen of consciousness. Invisible, but how? The depth of unconscious processing as inferred from different suppression techniques. 109.
go back to reference Ramsøy, T. Z., & Overgaard, M. (2004). Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 1–23.CrossRef Ramsøy, T. Z., & Overgaard, M. (2004). Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 1–23.CrossRef
go back to reference Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849.PubMedCrossRef Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shih, S. I. (2008). The attention cascade model and attentional blink. Cognitive psychology, 56(3), 210–236.PubMedCrossRef Shih, S. I. (2008). The attention cascade model and attentional blink. Cognitive psychology, 56(3), 210–236.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soto, D., & Silvanto, J. (2014). Reappraising the relationship between working memory and conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(10), 520–525.PubMedCrossRef Soto, D., & Silvanto, J. (2014). Reappraising the relationship between working memory and conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(10), 520–525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, B. K., Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2002). Voluntary and automatic attentional control of visual working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 64(5), 754–763.CrossRef Schmidt, B. K., Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2002). Voluntary and automatic attentional control of visual working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 64(5), 754–763.CrossRef
go back to reference Schönhammer, J. G., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Some effects of non-predictive cues on accuracy are mediated by feature-based attention. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 76.CrossRef Schönhammer, J. G., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Some effects of non-predictive cues on accuracy are mediated by feature-based attention. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 76.CrossRef
go back to reference Śmigasiewicz, K., Shalgi, S., Hsieh, S., Möller, F., Jaffe, S., Chang, C. C., & Verleger, R. (2010). Left visual-field advantage in the dual-stream RSVP task and reading-direction: A study in three nations. Neuropsychologia, 48(10), 2852–2860.PubMedCrossRef Śmigasiewicz, K., Shalgi, S., Hsieh, S., Möller, F., Jaffe, S., Chang, C. C., & Verleger, R. (2010). Left visual-field advantage in the dual-stream RSVP task and reading-direction: A study in three nations. Neuropsychologia, 48(10), 2852–2860.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soto, D., Mäntylä, T., & Silvanto, J. (2011). Working memory without consciousness. Current Biology, 21(22), R912–R913.PubMedCrossRef Soto, D., Mäntylä, T., & Silvanto, J. (2011). Working memory without consciousness. Current Biology, 21(22), R912–R913.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99.PubMedCrossRef Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. Control of cognitive processes: Attention and Performance XVIII, 105–124. Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. Control of cognitive processes: Attention and Performance XVIII, 105–124.
go back to reference Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101.PubMedCrossRef Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van den Bussche, E., Vermeiren, A., Desender, K., Gevers, W., Hughes, G., Verguts, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Disentangling conscious and unconscious processing: a subjective trial-based assessment approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. Van den Bussche, E., Vermeiren, A., Desender, K., Gevers, W., Hughes, G., Verguts, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Disentangling conscious and unconscious processing: a subjective trial-based assessment approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.
go back to reference Velichkovsky, B. B. (2017). Consciousness and working memory: Current trends and research perspectives. Consciousness and Cognition, 55, 35–45.PubMedCrossRef Velichkovsky, B. B. (2017). Consciousness and working memory: Current trends and research perspectives. Consciousness and Cognition, 55, 35–45.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Verleger, R., Möller, F., Kuniecki, M., Śmigasiewicz, K., Groppa, S., & Siebner, H. R. (2010). The left visual-field advantage in rapid visual presentation is amplified rather than reduced by posterior-parietal rTMS. Experimental Brain Research, 203(2), 355–365.PubMedCrossRef Verleger, R., Möller, F., Kuniecki, M., Śmigasiewicz, K., Groppa, S., & Siebner, H. R. (2010). The left visual-field advantage in rapid visual presentation is amplified rather than reduced by posterior-parietal rTMS. Experimental Brain Research, 203(2), 355–365.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Visser, T. A., Bischof, W. F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999). Attentional switching in spatial and nonspatial domains: Evidence from the attentional blink. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 458.CrossRef Visser, T. A., Bischof, W. F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999). Attentional switching in spatial and nonspatial domains: Evidence from the attentional blink. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 458.CrossRef
go back to reference Visser, T. A., Zuvic, S. M., Bischof, W. F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999). The attentional blink with targets in different spatial locations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6(3), 432–436.PubMedCrossRef Visser, T. A., Zuvic, S. M., Bischof, W. F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999). The attentional blink with targets in different spatial locations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6(3), 432–436.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vogel, E. K., Luck, S. J., & Shapiro, K. L. (1998). Electrophysiological evidence for a post-perceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1656.PubMed Vogel, E. K., Luck, S. J., & Shapiro, K. L. (1998). Electrophysiological evidence for a post-perceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1656.PubMed
go back to reference Weichselgartner, E., & Sperling, G. (1987). Dynamics of automatic and controlled visual attention. Science, 238(4828), 778–780.PubMedCrossRef Weichselgartner, E., & Sperling, G. (1987). Dynamics of automatic and controlled visual attention. Science, 238(4828), 778–780.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wyble, B., Bowman, H., & Potter, M. C. (2009). Categorically defined targets trigger spatiotemporal visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 324.PubMed Wyble, B., Bowman, H., & Potter, M. C. (2009). Categorically defined targets trigger spatiotemporal visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 324.PubMed
go back to reference Wyble, B., Folk, C., & Potter, M. C. (2013). Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(3), 861.PubMed Wyble, B., Folk, C., & Potter, M. C. (2013). Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(3), 861.PubMed
go back to reference Wyble, B., Potter, M. C., Bowman, H., & Nieuwenstein, M. (2011). Attentional episodes in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 488.CrossRef Wyble, B., Potter, M. C., Bowman, H., & Nieuwenstein, M. (2011). Attentional episodes in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 488.CrossRef
go back to reference Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2014). Attentional engagement is not sufficient to prevent spatial capture. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(1), 19–31.CrossRef Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2014). Attentional engagement is not sufficient to prevent spatial capture. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(1), 19–31.CrossRef
go back to reference Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2016). Attentional Capture and Engagement During the Attentional Blink: A “Camera” Metaphor of Attention. Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2016). Attentional Capture and Engagement During the Attentional Blink: A “Camera” Metaphor of Attention.
Metagegevens
Titel
An attentional blink in the absence of spatial attention: a cost of awareness?
Auteurs
Eyal Alef Ophir
Eyal Sherman
Dominique Lamy
Publicatiedatum
27-09-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1100-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2020

Psychological Research 4/2020 Naar de uitgave