Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

22-07-2024 | Research

Altered temporal awareness during Covid-19 pandemic

Auteurs: Şerife Leman Runyun, Virginie van Wassenhove, Fuat Balci

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2024

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic had profound effects on human well-being. A handful of studies have focused on how time perception was altered during the COVID-19 pandemic, while no study has tested whether temporal metacognition is also affected by the lockdown. We examined the impact of long-term social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic on the ability to monitor errors in timing performance. We recruited 1232 participants from 12 countries during lockdown, 211 of which were retested “post-pandemic” for within-group comparisons. We also tested a new group of 331 participants during the “post-pandemic” period and compared their data to those of 1232 participants tested during the lockdown (between-group comparison). Participants produced a 3600 ms target interval and assessed the magnitude and direction of their time production error. Both within and between-group comparisons showed reduced metric error monitoring performance during the lockdown, even after controlling for government-imposed stringency indices. A higher level of reported social isolation also predicted reduced temporal error monitoring ability. Participants produced longer duration during lockdown compared to post-lockdown (again controlling for government stringency indices). We reason that these effects may be underlain by altered biological and behavioral rhythms during social isolation experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic. Understanding these effects is crucial for a more complete characterization of the cognitive consequences of long-term social isolation.
Literatuur
go back to reference Akdoğan, B., & Balcı, F. (2017). Are you early or late? Temporal error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(3), 347–361.PubMedCrossRef Akdoğan, B., & Balcı, F. (2017). Are you early or late? Temporal error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(3), 347–361.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Andersson, M. A., Froese, P., & Bó, B. B. (2023). Out of time, out of mind: Multifaceted time perceptions and mental wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Time & Society, 0961463X231188786. Andersson, M. A., Froese, P., & Bó, B. B. (2023). Out of time, out of mind: Multifaceted time perceptions and mental wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Time & Society, 0961463X231188786.
go back to reference Balcı, F., Freestone, D., & Gallistel, C. R. (2009). Risk assessment in man and mouse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(7), 2459–2463.CrossRef Balcı, F., Freestone, D., & Gallistel, C. R. (2009). Risk assessment in man and mouse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(7), 2459–2463.CrossRef
go back to reference Balcı, F., Freestone, D., Simen, P., DeSouza, L., Cohen, J. D., & Holmes, P. (2011). Optimal Temporal Risk Assessment. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, pp. 5, 1–15. Balcı, F., Freestone, D., Simen, P., DeSouza, L., Cohen, J. D., & Holmes, P. (2011). Optimal Temporal Risk Assessment. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, pp. 5, 1–15.
go back to reference Balsam, P. D., Drew, M. R., & Yang, C. (2002). Timing at the start of associative learning. Learning and Motivation, 33(1), 141–155.CrossRef Balsam, P. D., Drew, M. R., & Yang, C. (2002). Timing at the start of associative learning. Learning and Motivation, 33(1), 141–155.CrossRef
go back to reference Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 755–765.PubMedCrossRef Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 755–765.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cole, S. W. (2015). The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 733.PubMedCrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cole, S. W. (2015). The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 733.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chaumon, M., Rioux, P. A., Herbst, S. K., Spiousas, I., Kübel, S. L., Hiroyasu, G., & van Wassenhove, E. M. (2022). V. The blursday database as a resource to study subjective temporalities during COVID-19. Nature Human Behaviour, 1–13. Chaumon, M., Rioux, P. A., Herbst, S. K., Spiousas, I., Kübel, S. L., Hiroyasu, G., & van Wassenhove, E. M. (2022). V. The blursday database as a resource to study subjective temporalities during COVID-19. Nature Human Behaviour, 1–13.
go back to reference Doenyas, C., Mutluer, T., Genç, E., & Balcı, F. (2019). Error monitoring in decision-making and timing is disrupted in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(2), 239–248.PubMedCrossRef Doenyas, C., Mutluer, T., Genç, E., & Balcı, F. (2019). Error monitoring in decision-making and timing is disrupted in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(2), 239–248.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duyan, A. Y., & Balcı, F. (2018). Numerical Error Monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1549–1555.CrossRef Duyan, A. Y., & Balcı, F. (2018). Numerical Error Monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1549–1555.CrossRef
go back to reference Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2019). Metric Error Monitoring in the Numerical estimates. Consciousness and Cognition, 67, 69–76.PubMedCrossRef Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2019). Metric Error Monitoring in the Numerical estimates. Consciousness and Cognition, 67, 69–76.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2020). Monitoring line length Reproduction errors (Vol. 77, p. 102831). Consciousness & Cognition. Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2020). Monitoring line length Reproduction errors (Vol. 77, p. 102831). Consciousness & Cognition.
go back to reference Fu, Z., Wu, D. A. J., Ross, I., Chung, J. M., Mamelak, A. N., Adolphs, R., & Rutishauser, U. (2019). Single-neuron correlates of error monitoring and post-error adjustments in human medial frontal cortex. Neuron, 101(1), 165–177.PubMedCrossRef Fu, Z., Wu, D. A. J., Ross, I., Chung, J. M., Mamelak, A. N., Adolphs, R., & Rutishauser, U. (2019). Single-neuron correlates of error monitoring and post-error adjustments in human medial frontal cortex. Neuron, 101(1), 165–177.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423(1), 52–77.PubMedCrossRef Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423(1), 52–77.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hale, T., et al. (2021). A global panel database of pandemic policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker). Nat Hum Behav, 5, 529–538.PubMedCrossRef Hale, T., et al. (2021). A global panel database of pandemic policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker). Nat Hum Behav, 5, 529–538.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Herzfeld, D. J., Kojima, Y., Soetedjo, R., & Shadmehr, R. (2018). Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nature Neuroscience, 21(5), 736–743.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Herzfeld, D. J., Kojima, Y., Soetedjo, R., & Shadmehr, R. (2018). Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nature Neuroscience, 21(5), 736–743.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Hiroyasu, E. M. G., Laje, R., Nomura, K., Spiousas, I., Hayashi, M. J., & Yotsumoto, Y. (2023). What the Blursday database reveals about longitudinal changes in temporal cognition: Effects of COVID-19 on spontaneous and paced finger-tapping task. Hiroyasu, E. M. G., Laje, R., Nomura, K., Spiousas, I., Hayashi, M. J., & Yotsumoto, Y. (2023). What the Blursday database reveals about longitudinal changes in temporal cognition: Effects of COVID-19 on spontaneous and paced finger-tapping task.
go back to reference Hu, N., Chen, A., Wang, Y., Li, Q., Xu, Z., & Long, Q. (2020). Acute stress impairs error monitoring and post-error adjustment. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(2), 162.CrossRef Hu, N., Chen, A., Wang, Y., Li, Q., Xu, Z., & Long, Q. (2020). Acute stress impairs error monitoring and post-error adjustment. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(2), 162.CrossRef
go back to reference Hwang, T. J., Rabheru, K., Peisah, C., Reichman, W., & Ikeda, M. (2020). Loneliness and social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Psychogeriatrics, 32(10), 1217–1220.PubMedCrossRef Hwang, T. J., Rabheru, K., Peisah, C., Reichman, W., & Ikeda, M. (2020). Loneliness and social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Psychogeriatrics, 32(10), 1217–1220.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ingram, J., Hand, C. J., & Maciejewski, G. (2021). Social isolation during COVID-19 lockdown impairs cognitive function. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35(4), 935–947.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ingram, J., Hand, C. J., & Maciejewski, G. (2021). Social isolation during COVID-19 lockdown impairs cognitive function. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35(4), 935–947.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kim, H. H. S., & Jung, J. H. (2021). Social isolation and psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-national analysis. The Gerontologist, 61(1), 103–113.PubMedCrossRef Kim, H. H. S., & Jung, J. H. (2021). Social isolation and psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-national analysis. The Gerontologist, 61(1), 103–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kononowicz, T. W., Roger, C., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019a). Temporal metacognition as the decoding of self-generated Brain Dynamics. Cerebral Cortex, 29(10), 4366–4380.PubMedCrossRef Kononowicz, T. W., Roger, C., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019a). Temporal metacognition as the decoding of self-generated Brain Dynamics. Cerebral Cortex, 29(10), 4366–4380.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lazic, S. E. (2010). The problem of pseudoreplication in neuroscientific studies: Is it affecting your analysis? BMC Neuroscience, 11, 1–17.CrossRef Lazic, S. E. (2010). The problem of pseudoreplication in neuroscientific studies: Is it affecting your analysis? BMC Neuroscience, 11, 1–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003). Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub-and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1583–1592.PubMedCrossRef Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003). Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub-and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1583–1592.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Matsumoto, K., Fujiwara, H., Araki, R., & Yabe, T. (2019). Post-weaning social isolation of mice: A putative animal model of developmental disorders. Journal of Pharmacological Sciences, 141(3), 111–118.PubMedCrossRef Matsumoto, K., Fujiwara, H., Araki, R., & Yabe, T. (2019). Post-weaning social isolation of mice: A putative animal model of developmental disorders. Journal of Pharmacological Sciences, 141(3), 111–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Temporal cognition: Connecting subjective time to perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 865.PubMedCrossRef Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Temporal cognition: Connecting subjective time to perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 865.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McEwen, B. S., Bowles, N. P., Gray, J. D., Hill, M. N., Hunter, R. G., Karatsoreos, I. N., & Nasca, C. (2015). Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 1353–1363.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef McEwen, B. S., Bowles, N. P., Gray, J. D., Hill, M. N., Hunter, R. G., Karatsoreos, I. N., & Nasca, C. (2015). Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 1353–1363.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Mumtaz, F., Khan, M. I., Zubair, M., & Dehpour, A. R. (2018). Neurobiology and consequences of social isolation stress in animal model—A comprehensive review. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy, 105, 1205–1222.CrossRef Mumtaz, F., Khan, M. I., Zubair, M., & Dehpour, A. R. (2018). Neurobiology and consequences of social isolation stress in animal model—A comprehensive review. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy, 105, 1205–1222.CrossRef
go back to reference Ogden, R. (2021). Why Covid-19 might be making us lose our sense of time…. The Cognitive Psychology Bulletin, 6(1). Ogden, R. (2021). Why Covid-19 might be making us lose our sense of time…. The Cognitive Psychology Bulletin, 6(1).
go back to reference Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Khalili-Mahani, N., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Buss, C., & Lupien, S. (2008). Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: Evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63(2), 234–240.PubMedCrossRef Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Khalili-Mahani, N., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Buss, C., & Lupien, S. (2008). Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: Evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63(2), 234–240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Error correction time without external error signals. Nature, 212(5060), 438–438.PubMedCrossRef Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Error correction time without external error signals. Nature, 212(5060), 438–438.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rahnev, D., Desender, K., Lee, A. L., Adler, W. T., Aguilar-Lleyda, D., Akdoğan, B., & Zylberberg, A. (2020). The confidence database. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 317–325.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rahnev, D., Desender, K., Lee, A. L., Adler, W. T., Aguilar-Lleyda, D., Akdoğan, B., & Zylberberg, A. (2020). The confidence database. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 317–325.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Rammsayer, T. H. (1999). Neuropharmacological evidence for different timing mechanisms in humans. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B, 52(3), 273–286. Rammsayer, T. H. (1999). Neuropharmacological evidence for different timing mechanisms in humans. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B, 52(3), 273–286.
go back to reference Read, S., Comas-Herrera, A., & Grundy, E. (2020). Social isolation and memory decline in later-life. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(2), 367–376.CrossRef Read, S., Comas-Herrera, A., & Grundy, E. (2020). Social isolation and memory decline in later-life. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(2), 367–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Rodeback, R. E., Hedges-Muncy, A., Hunt, I. J., Carbine, K. A., Steffen, P. R., & Larson, M. J. (2020). The association between experimentally induced stress, performance monitoring, and response inhibition: An event-related potential (ERP) analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, 189.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rodeback, R. E., Hedges-Muncy, A., Hunt, I. J., Carbine, K. A., Steffen, P. R., & Larson, M. J. (2020). The association between experimentally induced stress, performance monitoring, and response inhibition: An event-related potential (ERP) analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, 189.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (1978). Developing a measure of loneliness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 42, 290–294.PubMedCrossRef Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (1978). Developing a measure of loneliness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 42, 290–294.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., & Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring: Recent findings and theoretical perspectives. The Neuroscientist, 13(2), 160–172.PubMedCrossRef Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., & Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring: Recent findings and theoretical perspectives. The Neuroscientist, 13(2), 160–172.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ullsperger, M., Harsay, H. A., Wessel, J. R., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula. Brain Structure and Function, 214, 629–643.PubMedCrossRef Ullsperger, M., Harsay, H. A., Wessel, J. R., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula. Brain Structure and Function, 214, 629–643.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Hedger, K., Necka, E. A., Barakzai, A. K., & Norman, G. J. (2017). The influence of social stress on time perception and psychophysiological reactivity. Psychophysiology, 54(5), 706–712.PubMedCrossRef Van Hedger, K., Necka, E. A., Barakzai, A. K., & Norman, G. J. (2017). The influence of social stress on time perception and psychophysiological reactivity. Psychophysiology, 54(5), 706–712.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Wassenhove, V. (2022). Temporal disorientation and distortions during isolation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 104644. van Wassenhove, V. (2022). Temporal disorientation and distortions during isolation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 104644.
go back to reference Vijayraghavan, S., Wang, M., Birnbaum, S. G., Williams, G. V., & Arnsten, A. F. (2007). Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 10(3), 376–384.PubMedCrossRef Vijayraghavan, S., Wang, M., Birnbaum, S. G., Williams, G. V., & Arnsten, A. F. (2007). Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 10(3), 376–384.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wessel, J. R., Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M. (2011). Error awareness revisited: Accumulation of multimodal evidence from central and autonomic nervous systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 3021–3036.PubMedCrossRef Wessel, J. R., Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M. (2011). Error awareness revisited: Accumulation of multimodal evidence from central and autonomic nervous systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 3021–3036.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity, and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 7–12.PubMedCrossRef Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity, and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 7–12.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yallak, E., & Balcı, F. (2021). Metric error monitoring: Another generalized mechanism for magnitude representations? Cognition, 210, 104532.PubMedCrossRef Yallak, E., & Balcı, F. (2021). Metric error monitoring: Another generalized mechanism for magnitude representations? Cognition, 210, 104532.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yallak, E., & Balcı, F. (2022). Metric error monitoring for a cleaner record of timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(10), 1130.PubMed Yallak, E., & Balcı, F. (2022). Metric error monitoring for a cleaner record of timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(10), 1130.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Altered temporal awareness during Covid-19 pandemic
Auteurs
Şerife Leman Runyun
Virginie van Wassenhove
Fuat Balci
Publicatiedatum
22-07-2024
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2024
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0