Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2014

01-02-2014 | Original Paper

Altered Pre-reflective Sense of Agency in Autism Spectrum Disorders as Revealed by Reduced Intentional Binding

Auteurs: Marco Sperduti, Marie Pieron, Marion Leboyer, Tiziana Zalla

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 2/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are neurodevelopmental conditions that severely affect social interaction, communication and several behavioural and cognitive functions, such as planning and monitoring motor actions. A renewed interest in intrapersonal cognition has recently emerged suggesting a putative dissociation between impaired declarative processes, such as autobiographical memory, and spared implicit processes, such as the sense of agency (SoA) in ASDs. However, so far only a few studies have investigated the integrity of SoA using tasks exclusively tapping reflective mechanisms. Since pre-reflective processes of SoA are based on the same predictive internal models that are involved in planning and monitoring actions, we hypothesized that pre-reflective aspects of SoA, as measured by the intentional binding effect, would be altered in adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorders, relative to volunteers with typical development. Here, in accordance with our hypothesis, we report reduced IB in participants with ASDs.
Literatuur
go back to reference Allman, M. J., DeLeon, I. G., & Wearden, J. H. (2011). Psychophysical assessment of timing in individuals with autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116, 165–178.PubMedCrossRef Allman, M. J., DeLeon, I. G., & Wearden, J. H. (2011). Psychophysical assessment of timing in individuals with autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116, 165–178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatry Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th Edition DSM-IV-TR (Text Revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatry Association. American Psychiatry Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th Edition DSM-IV-TR (Text Revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatry Association.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore, S. J., Tavassoli, T., Calò, S., Thomas, R. M., Catmur, C., Frith, U., et al. (2006). Tactile sensitivity in Asperger syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 61, 5–13.PubMedCrossRef Blakemore, S. J., Tavassoli, T., Calò, S., Thomas, R. M., Catmur, C., Frith, U., et al. (2006). Tactile sensitivity in Asperger syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 61, 5–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D., & Frith, C. (2000). Why can’t you tickle yourself? Neuroreport, 11, R11–R16.PubMedCrossRef Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D., & Frith, C. (2000). Why can’t you tickle yourself? Neuroreport, 11, R11–R16.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buehner, M. J., & Humphreys, G. R. (2009). Causal binding of actions to their effects. Psychological Science, 20, 1221–1228.PubMedCrossRef Buehner, M. J., & Humphreys, G. R. (2009). Causal binding of actions to their effects. Psychological Science, 20, 1221–1228.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 589–599.PubMedCrossRef Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 589–599.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., et al. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 17825–17830.CrossRef Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., et al. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 17825–17830.CrossRef
go back to reference Collignon, O., Charbonneau, G., Peters, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., et al. (2012). Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex. 2012 Jun 20. Collignon, O., Charbonneau, G., Peters, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., et al. (2012). Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex. 2012 Jun 20.
go back to reference Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Specific and general autobiographical knowledge in adults with autism spectrum disorders: The role of personal goals. Memory, 17, 557–576.PubMedCrossRef Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Specific and general autobiographical knowledge in adults with autism spectrum disorders: The role of personal goals. Memory, 17, 557–576.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2010). Brief report: Self-defining and everyday autobiographical memories in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 383–391.PubMedCrossRef Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2010). Brief report: Self-defining and everyday autobiographical memories in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 383–391.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cravo, A. M., Claessens, P. M., & Baldo, M. V. (2009). Voluntary action and causality in temporal binding. Experimental Brain Research, 199, 95–99.PubMedCrossRef Cravo, A. M., Claessens, P. M., & Baldo, M. V. (2009). Voluntary action and causality in temporal binding. Experimental Brain Research, 199, 95–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cravo, A. M., Claessens, P. M., & Baldo, M. V. (2011). The relation between action, predictability and temporal contiguity in temporal binding. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 136, 157–166.CrossRef Cravo, A. M., Claessens, P. M., & Baldo, M. V. (2011). The relation between action, predictability and temporal contiguity in temporal binding. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 136, 157–166.CrossRef
go back to reference David, N., Gawronski, A., Santos, N. S., Huff, W., Lehnhardt, F. G., Newen, A., et al. (2008a). Dissociation between key processes of social cognition in autism: Impaired mentalizing but intact sense of agency. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 593–605.PubMedCrossRef David, N., Gawronski, A., Santos, N. S., Huff, W., Lehnhardt, F. G., Newen, A., et al. (2008a). Dissociation between key processes of social cognition in autism: Impaired mentalizing but intact sense of agency. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 593–605.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference David, N., Newen, A., & Vogeley, K. (2008b). The ‘‘sense of agency’’ and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 523–534.PubMedCrossRef David, N., Newen, A., & Vogeley, K. (2008b). The ‘‘sense of agency’’ and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 523–534.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., et al. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2(3), 264–287.PubMedCrossRef DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., et al. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2(3), 264–287.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Desantis, A., Hughes, G., Waszak, F. (2012). Intentional binding is driven by the mere presence of an action and not by motor prediction. PLoS One, 7, e29557. Desantis, A., Hughes, G., Waszak, F. (2012). Intentional binding is driven by the mere presence of an action and not by motor prediction. PLoS One, 7, e29557.
go back to reference Desantis, A., Roussel, C., & Waszak, F. (2011). On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1211–1220.PubMedCrossRef Desantis, A., Roussel, C., & Waszak, F. (2011). On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1211–1220.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dewey, D., Cantell, M., & Crawford, S. G. (2007). Motor and gestural performance in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental coordination disorder, and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(2), 246–256.PubMedCrossRef Dewey, D., Cantell, M., & Crawford, S. G. (2007). Motor and gestural performance in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental coordination disorder, and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(2), 246–256.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ebert, J. P., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Time warp: Authorship shapes the perceived timing of actions and events. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 481–489.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Ebert, J. P., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Time warp: Authorship shapes the perceived timing of actions and events. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 481–489.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, K., & Wohlschläger, A. (2007). Intentions and expectations in temporal binding. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 255–264.PubMedCrossRef Engbert, K., & Wohlschläger, A. (2007). Intentions and expectations in temporal binding. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 255–264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, K., Wohlschläger, A., & Haggard, P. (2008). Who is causing what? The sense of agency is relational and efferent-triggered. Cognition, 107, 693–704.PubMedCrossRef Engbert, K., Wohlschläger, A., & Haggard, P. (2008). Who is causing what? The sense of agency is relational and efferent-triggered. Cognition, 107, 693–704.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, K., Wohlschläger, A., Thomas, R., & Haggard, P. (2007). Agency, subjective time, and other minds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1261–1268.PubMed Engbert, K., Wohlschläger, A., Thomas, R., & Haggard, P. (2007). Agency, subjective time, and other minds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1261–1268.PubMed
go back to reference Enticott, P. G., Bradshaw, J. L., Iansek, R., Tonge, B. J., & Rinehart, N. J. (2009). Electrophysiological signs of supplementary-motor-area deficits in high-functioning autism but not Asperger syndrome: An examination of internally cued movement-related potentials. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51, 787–791.PubMedCrossRef Enticott, P. G., Bradshaw, J. L., Iansek, R., Tonge, B. J., & Rinehart, N. J. (2009). Electrophysiological signs of supplementary-motor-area deficits in high-functioning autism but not Asperger syndrome: An examination of internally cued movement-related potentials. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51, 787–791.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Falter, C. M., Noreika, V., Wearden, J. H., & Bailey, A. J. (2012). More consistent, yet less sensitive: Interval timing in autism spectrum disorders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 65, 2093–2107.CrossRef Falter, C. M., Noreika, V., Wearden, J. H., & Bailey, A. J. (2012). More consistent, yet less sensitive: Interval timing in autism spectrum disorders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 65, 2093–2107.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
go back to reference Gil, S., Chambres, P., Hyvert, C., Fanget, M., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). Children with autism spectrum disorders have “the working raw material” for time perception. PLoS One, 7, e49116.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Gil, S., Chambres, P., Hyvert, C., Fanget, M., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). Children with autism spectrum disorders have “the working raw material” for time perception. PLoS One, 7, e49116.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gillberg, C., Gillberg, C., Raåstam, M., & Wentz, E. (2001). The Asperger syndrome (and high-functioning autism) diagnostic interview (ASDI): A preliminary study of a new structured clinical interview. Autism, 5, 57–66.PubMedCrossRef Gillberg, C., Gillberg, C., Raåstam, M., & Wentz, E. (2001). The Asperger syndrome (and high-functioning autism) diagnostic interview (ASDI): A preliminary study of a new structured clinical interview. Autism, 5, 57–66.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Glazebrook, C. M., Gonzalez, D., Hansen, S., & Elliott, D. (2009). The role of vision for online control of manual aiming movements in persons with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13, 411–433.PubMedCrossRef Glazebrook, C. M., Gonzalez, D., Hansen, S., & Elliott, D. (2009). The role of vision for online control of manual aiming movements in persons with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 13, 411–433.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gomes, G. (2002). The interpretation of Libet’s results on the timing of conscious events: A commentary. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 221–230.PubMedCrossRef Gomes, G. (2002). The interpretation of Libet’s results on the timing of conscious events: A commentary. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 221–230.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gowen, E., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Motor abilities in autism: A review using a computational context. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 323–344.PubMedCrossRef Gowen, E., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Motor abilities in autism: A review using a computational context. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 323–344.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grynszpan, O., Nadel, J., Martin, J. C., Simonin, J., Bailleul, P., Wang, Y., et al. (2012). Self-monitoring of gaze in high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1642–1650.PubMedCrossRef Grynszpan, O., Nadel, J., Martin, J. C., Simonin, J., Bailleul, P., Wang, Y., et al. (2012). Self-monitoring of gaze in high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1642–1650.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Haggard, P., & Clark, S. (2003). Intentional action: Conscious experience and neural prediction. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 695–707.PubMedCrossRef Haggard, P., & Clark, S. (2003). Intentional action: Conscious experience and neural prediction. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 695–707.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382–385.PubMedCrossRef Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382–385.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henderson, H., Schwartz, C., Mundy, P., Burnette, C., Sutton, S., Zahka, N., et al. (2006). Response monitoring, the error-related negativity, and differences in social behavior in autism. Brain and Cognition, 61, 96–109.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Henderson, H., Schwartz, C., Mundy, P., Burnette, C., Sutton, S., Zahka, N., et al. (2006). Response monitoring, the error-related negativity, and differences in social behavior in autism. Brain and Cognition, 61, 96–109.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hughes, G., Desantis, A., & Waszak, F. (2013). Attenuation of auditory N1 results from identity-specific action-effect prediction. European Journal of Neuroscience,. doi:10.1111/ejn.12120.PubMed Hughes, G., Desantis, A., & Waszak, F. (2013). Attenuation of auditory N1 results from identity-specific action-effect prediction. European Journal of Neuroscience,. doi:10.​1111/​ejn.​12120.PubMed
go back to reference Hughes, G., & Waszak, F. (2011). ERP correlates of action effect prediction and visual sensory attenuation in voluntary action. Neuroimage, 56, 1632–1640.PubMedCrossRef Hughes, G., & Waszak, F. (2011). ERP correlates of action effect prediction and visual sensory attenuation in voluntary action. Neuroimage, 56, 1632–1640.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, G. R., & Buehner, M. J. (2009). Magnitude estimation reveals temporal binding at super-second intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1542–1549.PubMed Humphreys, G. R., & Buehner, M. J. (2009). Magnitude estimation reveals temporal binding at super-second intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1542–1549.PubMed
go back to reference Humphreys, G. R., & Buehner, M. J. (2010). Temporal binding of action and effect in interval reproduction. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 465–470.PubMedCrossRef Humphreys, G. R., & Buehner, M. J. (2010). Temporal binding of action and effect in interval reproduction. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 465–470.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kaiser, M. D., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A review and synthesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 761–777.CrossRef Kaiser, M. D., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A review and synthesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 761–777.CrossRef
go back to reference Klein, S. B., & Gangi, C. E. (2010). The multiplicity of self: Neuropsychological evidence and its implications for the self as a construct in psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 1–15.PubMedCrossRef Klein, S. B., & Gangi, C. E. (2010). The multiplicity of self: Neuropsychological evidence and its implications for the self as a construct in psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 1–15.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kühn, S., Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2012). Feeling in control: Neural correlates of experience of agency. Cortex, 49, 1935–1942.PubMedCrossRef Kühn, S., Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2012). Feeling in control: Neural correlates of experience of agency. Cortex, 49, 1935–1942.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., Sadek, S. A., Pasco, G., Wheelwright, S. J., et al. (2010). Atypical neural self-representation in autism. Brain, 133, 611–624.PubMedCrossRef Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., Sadek, S. A., Pasco, G., Wheelwright, S. J., et al. (2010). Atypical neural self-representation in autism. Brain, 133, 611–624.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Magnée, M. J., de Gelder, B., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2011). Multisensory integration and attention in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from event-related potentials. PLoS One, 6, e24196.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Magnée, M. J., de Gelder, B., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2011). Multisensory integration and attention in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from event-related potentials. PLoS One, 6, e24196.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maister, L., & Plaisted-Grant, K. C. (2011). Time perception and its relationship to memory in Autism Spectrum Conditions. Developmental Science, 14, 1311–1322.PubMedCrossRef Maister, L., & Plaisted-Grant, K. C. (2011). Time perception and its relationship to memory in Autism Spectrum Conditions. Developmental Science, 14, 1311–1322.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & Prior, M. (2003). The reach-to-grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Science, 358, 393–403.CrossRef Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & Prior, M. (2003). The reach-to-grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Science, 358, 393–403.CrossRef
go back to reference Martin, J. S., Poirier, M., & Bowler, D. M. (2010). Brief report: Impaired temporal reproduction performance in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 640–646.PubMedCrossRef Martin, J. S., Poirier, M., & Bowler, D. M. (2010). Brief report: Impaired temporal reproduction performance in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 640–646.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martineau, J., Schmitz, C., Assaiante, C., Blanc, R., & Barthélémy, C. (2004). Impairment of a cortical event-related desynchronisation during a bimanual load-lifting task in children with autistic disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 367, 298–303.PubMedCrossRef Martineau, J., Schmitz, C., Assaiante, C., Blanc, R., & Barthélémy, C. (2004). Impairment of a cortical event-related desynchronisation during a bimanual load-lifting task in children with autistic disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 367, 298–303.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miele, D. B., Wager, T. D., Mitchell, J. P., & Metcalfe, J. (2011). Dissociating neural correlates of action monitoring and metacognition of agency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3620–3636.PubMedCrossRef Miele, D. B., Wager, T. D., Mitchell, J. P., & Metcalfe, J. (2011). Dissociating neural correlates of action monitoring and metacognition of agency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3620–3636.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, J. W., & Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Sense of agency in health and disease: A review of cue integration approaches. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 59–68.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Moore, J. W., & Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Sense of agency in health and disease: A review of cue integration approaches. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 59–68.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, J., & Haggard, P. (2008). Awareness of action: Inference and prediction. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 136–144.PubMedCrossRef Moore, J., & Haggard, P. (2008). Awareness of action: Inference and prediction. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 136–144.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, J. W., Ruge, D., Wenke, D., Rothwell, J., & Haggard, P. (2010). Disrupting the experience of control in the human brain: Pre-supplementary motor area contributes to the sense of agency. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 2503–2509.PubMedCrossRef Moore, J. W., Ruge, D., Wenke, D., Rothwell, J., & Haggard, P. (2010). Disrupting the experience of control in the human brain: Pre-supplementary motor area contributes to the sense of agency. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 2503–2509.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, J. W., Wegner, D. M., & Haggard, P. (2009). Modulating the sense of agency with external cues. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 1056–1064.PubMedCrossRef Moore, J. W., Wegner, D. M., & Haggard, P. (2009). Modulating the sense of agency with external cues. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 1056–1064.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mostofsky, S. H., Goldberg, M. C., Landa, R. J., & Denckla, M. B. (2000). Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 752–759.PubMedCrossRef Mostofsky, S. H., Goldberg, M. C., Landa, R. J., & Denckla, M. B. (2000). Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 752–759.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nazarali, N., Glazebrook, C. M., & Elliott, D. (2009). Movement planning and reprogramming in individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1401–1411.PubMedCrossRef Nazarali, N., Glazebrook, C. M., & Elliott, D. (2009). Movement planning and reprogramming in individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1401–1411.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Obhi, S. S., & Hall, P. (2011a). Sense of agency and intentional binding in joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 655–662.PubMedCrossRef Obhi, S. S., & Hall, P. (2011a). Sense of agency and intentional binding in joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 655–662.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Obhi, S. S., & Hall, P. (2011b). Sense of agency in joint action: Influence of human and computer co-actors. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 663–670.PubMedCrossRef Obhi, S. S., & Hall, P. (2011b). Sense of agency in joint action: Influence of human and computer co-actors. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 663–670.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1998). Understanding intention in normal development and autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 337–348.CrossRef Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1998). Understanding intention in normal development and autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 337–348.CrossRef
go back to reference Picard, N., & Strick, P. L. (1996). Motor areas of the medial wall: A review of their location and functional activation. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 342–353.PubMedCrossRef Picard, N., & Strick, P. L. (1996). Motor areas of the medial wall: A review of their location and functional activation. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 342–353.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pockett, S., & Miller, A. (2007). The rotating spot method of timing subjective events. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 241–254.PubMedCrossRef Pockett, S., & Miller, A. (2007). The rotating spot method of timing subjective events. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 241–254.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rinehart, N. J., Bellgrove, M. A., Tonge, B. J., Brereton, A. V., Howells-Rankin, D., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006a). An examination of movement kinematics in young people with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder: Further evidence for a motor planning deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 757–767.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Rinehart, N. J., Bellgrove, M. A., Tonge, B. J., Brereton, A. V., Howells-Rankin, D., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006a). An examination of movement kinematics in young people with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder: Further evidence for a motor planning deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 757–767.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). Movement preparation in high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder: A serial choice reaction time task involving motor reprogramming. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 79–88.PubMedCrossRef Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). Movement preparation in high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder: A serial choice reaction time task involving motor reprogramming. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 79–88.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Iansek, R., Enticott, P. G., & Johnson, K. A. (2006b). Movement-related potentials in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48, 272–277.PubMedCrossRef Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Iansek, R., Enticott, P. G., & Johnson, K. A. (2006b). Movement-related potentials in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48, 272–277.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Russo, N., Foxe, J. J., Brandwein, A. B., Altschuler, T., Gomes, H., & Molholm, S. (2010). Multisensory processing in children with autism: High-density electrical mapping of auditory-somatosensory integration. Autism Research, 3, 253–267.PubMedCrossRef Russo, N., Foxe, J. J., Brandwein, A. B., Altschuler, T., Gomes, H., & Molholm, S. (2010). Multisensory processing in children with autism: High-density electrical mapping of auditory-somatosensory integration. Autism Research, 3, 253–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Santesso, D. L., Drmic, I. E., Jetha, M. K., Bryson, S. E., Goldberg, J. O., Hall, G. B., et al. (2010). An event-related source localization study of response monitoring and social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Psychophysiology, 48, 241–251.CrossRef Santesso, D. L., Drmic, I. E., Jetha, M. K., Bryson, S. E., Goldberg, J. O., Hall, G. B., et al. (2010). An event-related source localization study of response monitoring and social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Psychophysiology, 48, 241–251.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmitz, C., Martineau, J., Barthélémy, C., & Assaiante, C. (2003). Motor control and children with autism: Deficit of anticipatory function? Neuroscience Letters, 348, 17–20.PubMedCrossRef Schmitz, C., Martineau, J., Barthélémy, C., & Assaiante, C. (2003). Motor control and children with autism: Deficit of anticipatory function? Neuroscience Letters, 348, 17–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739.PubMedCrossRef Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–273.PubMedCrossRef Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–273.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spence, C., & Squire, S. (2003). Multisensory integration: Maintaining the perception of synchrony. Current Biology, 13, R519.PubMedCrossRef Spence, C., & Squire, S. (2003). Multisensory integration: Maintaining the perception of synchrony. Current Biology, 13, R519.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sperduti, M., Delaveau, P., Fossati, P., & Nadel, J. (2011). Different brain structures related to self- and external-agency attribution: A brief review and meta-analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 216, 151–157.PubMedCrossRef Sperduti, M., Delaveau, P., Fossati, P., & Nadel, J. (2011). Different brain structures related to self- and external-agency attribution: A brief review and meta-analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 216, 151–157.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Strother, L., House, K. A., & Obhi, S. S. (2010). Subjective agency and awareness of shared actions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 12–20.PubMedCrossRef Strother, L., House, K. A., & Obhi, S. S. (2010). Subjective agency and awareness of shared actions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 12–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Lindner, A. (2009). Me or not me–an optimal integration of agency cues? Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 1065–1068.PubMedCrossRef Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Lindner, A. (2009). Me or not me–an optimal integration of agency cues? Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 1065–1068.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008). Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 219–239.PubMedCrossRef Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008). Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 219–239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Szelag, E., Kowalska, J., Galkowski, T., & Pöppel, E. (2004). Temporal processing deficits in high-functioning children with autism. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 269–282.PubMedCrossRef Szelag, E., Kowalska, J., Galkowski, T., & Pöppel, E. (2004). Temporal processing deficits in high-functioning children with autism. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 269–282.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Takahata, K., Takahashi, H., Maeda, T., Umeda, S., Suhara, T., Mimura, M., et al. (2012). It’s not my fault: Postdictive modulation of intentional binding by monetary gains and losses. PLoS One, 7, e53421.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Takahata, K., Takahashi, H., Maeda, T., Umeda, S., Suhara, T., Mimura, M., et al. (2012). It’s not my fault: Postdictive modulation of intentional binding by monetary gains and losses. PLoS One, 7, e53421.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thakkar, K. N., Polli, F. E., Joseph, R. M., Tuch, D. S., Hadjikhani, N., Barton, J. J., et al. (2008). Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Brain, 131, 2464–2478.PubMedCrossRef Thakkar, K. N., Polli, F. E., Joseph, R. M., Tuch, D. S., Hadjikhani, N., Barton, J. J., et al. (2008). Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Brain, 131, 2464–2478.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vlamings, P. H., Jonkman, L. M., Hoeksma, M. R., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2008). Reduced error monitoring in children with autism spectrum disorder: An ERP study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 399–406.PubMedCrossRef Vlamings, P. H., Jonkman, L. M., Hoeksma, M. R., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2008). Reduced error monitoring in children with autism spectrum disorder: An ERP study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 399–406.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., & Haggard, P. (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: A specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain, 133, 3104–3112.PubMedCrossRef Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., & Haggard, P. (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: A specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain, 133, 3104–3112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wallace, G. L., & Happé, F. (2008). Time perception in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 447–455.CrossRef Wallace, G. L., & Happé, F. (2008). Time perception in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 447–455.CrossRef
go back to reference Waszak, F., Cardoso-Leite, P., & Hughes, G. (2012). Action effect anticipation: Neurophysiological basis and functional consequences. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 943–959.PubMedCrossRef Waszak, F., Cardoso-Leite, P., & Hughes, G. (2012). Action effect anticipation: Neurophysiological basis and functional consequences. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 943–959.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (2000). Echelle clinique de mémoire de Wechsler-révisé: Manuel. Paris: Centre de Psychologie Appliquée. Wechsler, D. (2000). Echelle clinique de mémoire de Wechsler-révisé: Manuel. Paris: Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
go back to reference Weiss, C., Herwig, A., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The self in action effects: Selective attenuation of self-generated sounds. Cognition, 121, 207–218.PubMedCrossRef Weiss, C., Herwig, A., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The self in action effects: Selective attenuation of self-generated sounds. Cognition, 121, 207–218.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 287–295.CrossRef Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 287–295.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., & Miall, R. C. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks, 9, 1265–1279.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., & Miall, R. C. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks, 9, 1265–1279.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zalla, T., Daprati, E., Sav, A. M., Chaste, P., Nico, D., & Leboyer, M. (2010a). Memory for self-performed actions in individuals with Asperger syndrome. PLoS One, 5, e13370.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Zalla, T., Daprati, E., Sav, A. M., Chaste, P., Nico, D., & Leboyer, M. (2010a). Memory for self-performed actions in individuals with Asperger syndrome. PLoS One, 5, e13370.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zalla, T., Labruyere, N., Clément, A., & Georgieff, N. (2010b). Predicting ensuing actions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 809–819.PubMedCrossRef Zalla, T., Labruyere, N., Clément, A., & Georgieff, N. (2010b). Predicting ensuing actions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 809–819.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zalla, T., Labruyere, N., & Georgieff, N. (2006). Goal-directed action representation in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 527–540.PubMedCrossRef Zalla, T., Labruyere, N., & Georgieff, N. (2006). Goal-directed action representation in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 527–540.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Altered Pre-reflective Sense of Agency in Autism Spectrum Disorders as Revealed by Reduced Intentional Binding
Auteurs
Marco Sperduti
Marie Pieron
Marion Leboyer
Tiziana Zalla
Publicatiedatum
01-02-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 2/2014
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1891-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2014

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2014 Naar de uitgave