Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 6/2022

Open Access 28-06-2021 | Brief Report

Alexithymic But Not Autistic Traits Impair Prosocial Behavior

Auteurs: Alexander Lischke, Harald J. Freyberger, Hans J. Grabe, Anett Mau-Moeller, Rike Pahnke

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 6/2022

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Social impairments are a core feature of autism-spectrum disorders. However, there is a considerable variability in these impairments. Most autistic individuals show large impairments in social functioning but some autistic individuals show small impairments in social functioning. The variability of these impairments has been attributed to the presence or absence of alexithymia. To address this issue, we capitalized on the fact that alexithymic and autistic traits are broadly distributed in the population. This allowed us to investigate how alexithymic and autistic traits affect social functioning in healthy individuals. Healthy individuals showed impairments on a resource-allocation task that were due to alexithymic but not autistic traits. These findings suggest that alexithymic rather than autistic traits impair prosocial behavior across the autism-spectrum.
Opmerkingen
Harald J. Freyberger—Deceased.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Autism-spectrum disorder (ASD) is a clinical condition that is characterized by impairments in social cognition and social interaction (APA, 2013). Although social impairments are common among autistic individuals (Velikonja et al., 2019), there is a considerable variability in these impairments. The processing of others’ emotions, for instance, varies considerably among autistic individuals (Harmset al., 2010). Most autistic individuals show large impairments in emotion processing but some autistic individuals show small impairments in emotion processing. The variability of these and other impairments may depend on the presence or absence of alexithymia (Bird & Cook, 2013). Alexithymia is a non-clinical condition that is characterized by difficulties in identifying and describing one’s own emotions (Nemiah et al., 1976). Considering that emotions serve as guidance in many social contexts (Keltner & Haidt, 1999), it is not surprising that alexithymia is often associated with impairments in social cognition and social interaction (Grynberg et al., 2018). Alexithymia is quite prevalent among autistic individuals (Kinnaird et al., 2019), implying that autistic individuals with high levels of alexithymia may be more impaired in social cognition and social interaction than autistic individuals with low levels of alexithymia. Autistic individuals with high levels of alexithymia show indeed more impairments in social cognition than autistic individuals with low levels of alexithymia. Emotion recognition or empathetic responding, for instance, is more impaired in autistic individuals with high than low levels of alexithymia (Bird et al., 2010; Cook et al., 2013; Silani et al., 2008). We, thus, assume that impairments in social interaction are also more pronounced among autistic individual with high than low levels of alexithymia.
To test this assumption, we capitalized on the fact that autistic and alexithymic traits are broadly distributed in the population (Franz et al., 2008; Ruzich et al., 2015). This allowed us to investigate how autistic and alexithymic traits impair social interaction in healthy individuals. Impairments in social interaction can be modelled with economic tasks that operationalize social interaction in terms of prosocial behavior (King-Casas & Chiu, 2012). Following this approach, we administered a resource allocation task to a sample of healthy individuals whose autistic and alexithymic traits had been determined with personality questionnaires. Similar as in previous investigations (Brewer et al., 2015; Cook et al., 2013), we performed correlation and regression analyses to investigate associations between task performance and personality traits. Assuming that task performance would be more impaired by alexithymic than by autistic traits (Bird & Cook, 2013), we expected alexithymic rather than autistic traits to be negatively associated with prosocial behavior on the resource allocation task.

Method

Participants

Seventy-four healthy individuals (ethnicity: Caucasian, age range: 18–35 years, educational level: higher education) participated in the study. None of the participants was or had been in psychotherapeutic or psychopharmacological treatment. A power analysis with G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) indicated that the number of participants was large enough to detect medium sized associations between prosocial behavior and autistic or alexithmic traits in the planned analyses (correlation analyses, one-sided, and regression analyses, two-sided: α = 0.05, 1 − β = 80, r = 0.30, f2 = 0.15). All participants provided written informed consent to the study protocol that was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Rostock and carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Questionnaires

We used in-house questionnaires for the assessment of participants’ demographical characteristics (age, sex, education) and established questionnaires for the assessment of participants’ psychological characteristics (psychopathology, autism, alexithymia). Psychopathological symptoms were assessed with the depression and anxiety scales of the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis, 2000), autistic traits were assessed with the Autism Spectrum Quotient 10 (AQ-10; Allison et al., 2012; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) and alexithymic traits were assessed with the Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (TAS-20; Bagby et al., 1994a, 1994b; Parker et al., 2003).

Task

We used the Social Value Orientation test (Murphy et al., 2011), a resource allocation task, to assess participants’ pro-social behavior via a computer interface (Lischke et al., 2018). The SVO comprised six items with a choice over a defined continuum of self-other payoff allocations (see Fig. 1). Participants had to select payoff allocations that reflected their most preferred payoffs for themselves and another participant whose identity remained anonymous throughout the study. On basis of these selections, the inverse ratio between the mean payoffs for the self and the other was calculated. The resulting index, the social value orientation angle (SVO-A), reflected participants’ preferences for pro-social allocations (i.e., allocations with higher payoffs for the other than for the self) as compared to anti-social allocations (i.e., allocations with lower payoffs for the other than for the self). Higher SVO-A values indicated that participants displayed prosocial behavior (upper limit: 61.39°) and lower SVO-A values indicated that participants displayed anti-social behavior (lower limit: − 16.26°).

Statistical Analysis

We used SPSS 22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) for all analyses. Our preliminary analyses of participants’ task performance revealed invalid allocation selections (i.e. allocations outside of the range of possible allocations). These invalid selections compromised the determination of the social value orientation index (Murphy et al., 2011), limiting the number of participants that could be considered in our main analyses (n = 67; see Table 1). Our main analyses comprised correlation and regression analyses, which were performed with bootstrapping (10,000 samples) to control for deviations from normality (Wright et al., 2011). Whereas the correlation analyses allowed us to explore associations between participants’ personality traits and participants’ task performance, the regression analyses allowed us to investigate associations between participants’ personality traits and participants’ task performance in more detail. To rule out that the results of the correlation and regression analyses were affected by other participant characteristics than participants’ personality traits (Hendryx et al., 1991; Kanai et al., 2011), we controlled for differences in participants’ age, sex, depression and anxiety in all analyses. We set the significance level for these analyses at p ≤ 0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons) and determined significance values (p), effect size measures (r, R2, ΔR2, B, z, q) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) to facilitate the interpretation of the corresponding results.
Table 1
Participant characteristics
 
M (SE M)/N
Sex (m/f)
33/34
Age (years)
26.10 (0.50)
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
0.53 (0.06)
Depression (BSI-DEP)
0.35 (0.05)
Alexithymia (TAS-20)
43.03 (1.22)
Autism (AQ-10)
2.19 (0.14)
Cooperation (SVO-A)
32.16 (1.29)
m male, f female, BSI-ANX Brief Symptom Inventory—Anxiety Scale (Derogatis, 2000), BSI-DEP Brief Symptom Inventory—Depression Scale (Derogatis, 2000), TAS-20 Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (Bagby et al., 1994a, 1994b; Parker et al., 2003), AQ-10 Autism Spectrum Quotient 10 (Allison et al., 2012; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), SVO-A Social Value Orientation—Angle (Murphy et al., 2011)

Results

We run a series of correlation analyses to explore associations between participants’ personality traits and participants’ task performance. To control for participant characteristics that may affect these associations (age, sex, depression, anxiety), we performed partial instead of full correlations. We found a positive association between participants’ autistic and alexithymic traits (r(61) = 0.32, p = 0.011, 95% CI [0.06, 0.53]). Whereas participants’ autistic symptoms were not associated with participants’ prosocial behavior (r(61) = 0.07, p = 0.591, 95% CI [− 0.15, 0.24], see Fig. 2), participants’ alexithymic symptoms were negatively associated with participants’ prosocial behavior (r(61) = − 0.29, p = 0.022, 95% CI [− 0.50, − 0.04], see Fig. 1). A formal comparison of the correlation coefficients that were obtained in these analyses confirmed that participants’ autistic and alexithymic traits were differentially associated with participants’ prosocial behavior (z = 2.52, p = 0.006, q = 0.37).
We run a series of regression analyses to further investigate the associations between participants’ personality traits and participants’ task performance. To control for participant characteristics that may affect these associations (age, sex, depression, anxiety), we entered these participant characteristics before participants’ autistic and alexithymic traits into the respective regression models. Whereas participants’ autistic traits were entered before participants’ alexithymic traits into one regression model (model one), participants’ autistic traits were entered after participants’ alexithymic traits into another regression model (model two). Varying the order of participants’ alexithymic and autistic traits in the regression models allowed us to control the close association between these traits (Brewer et al., 2015; Cook et al., 2013). Regardless whether we entered participants’ autistic traits before or after participants’ alexithymic traits into the regression model (see Table 2), we found no association between participants’ autistic traits and participants’ prosocial behavior (model one (step 3): B = 1.55, 95% CI [− 0.46, 3.17], t(60) = 1.39, p = 0.090; model two (step 2): B = 0.60, 95% CI [− 1.45, 2.14], t(61) = 0.54, p = 0.461). As a consequence, participants’ autistic traits failed to account for a substantial proportion of participants’ prosocial behavior (model one (step 3): ΔR2 = 0.03, ΔF(1, 60) = 1.94, p = 0.169; model two (step 2): ΔR2 = 0.00, ΔF(1, 61) = 0.29, p = 0.591). We found, however, a negative association between participants’ alexithymic traits and participants’ prosocial behavior [model one (step 2): B = − 0.35, 95% CI [− 0.59, − 0.07], t(61) = − 2.35, p = 0.022; model two (step 3): B = − 0.42, 95% CI [− 0.68, − 0.10], t(60) = − 2.69, p = 0.009]. The association emerged regardless whether participants’ alexithymic traits were entered before or after participants’ autistic traits (see Table 2). Consequently, participants’ alexithymic traits accounted for a substantial proportion of participants’ prosocial behavior [model one (step 2): ΔR2 = 0.08, ΔF(1, 61) = 5.50, p = 0.022; model two (step 3): ΔR2 = 0.10, ΔF(1, 60) = 7.21, p = 0.009].
Table 2
Associations between participants’ prosocial behavior and participants’ autistic or alexithymic traits
Model one
Prosocial behavior (SVO-A)
Model two
Prosocial behavior (SVO-A)
B
SE B
95% CI
t
p
B
SE B
95% CI
t
p
Step one
     
Step one
     
Sex
− 0.38
0.36
[− 1.03, 0.33]
− 1.15
0.293
Sex
− 0.38
0.37
[− 1.07, 0.38]
− 1.15
0.326
Age (years)
1.46
2.53
[− 3.53, 6.46]
0.55
0.565
Age (years)
1.46
2.68
[− 3.91, 6.64]
0.55
0.604
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
− 2.25
3.17
[− 8.73, 3.70]
− 0.75
0.486
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
− 2.25
3.10
[− 8.00, 3.86]
− 0.75
0.503
Depression (BSI-DEP)
− 0.94
5.57
[− 13.08, 8.53]
− 0.27
0.867
Depression (BSI-DEP)
− 0.94
5.64
[− 13.67, 8.25]
− 0.27
0.860
Step two
     
Step two
     
Sex
− 0.46
0.36
[− 1.12, 0.27]
− 1.45
0.217
Sex
− 0.36
0.37
[− 1.06, 0.41]
− 1.11
0.331
Age (years)
3.27
2.76
[− 2.53, 8.53]
1.22
0.259
Age (years)
1.45
2.71
[− 3.91, 6.79]
0.54
0.593
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
0.83
3.51
[− 6.35, 7.50]
0.26
0.808
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
− 2.49
3.21
[− 8.26, 4.09]
− 0.81
0.463
Depression (BSI-DEP)
0.79
5.29
[− 10.84, 10.01]
0.23
0.856
Depression (BSI-DEP)
− 0.98
5.64
[− 13.92, 8.26]
− 0.28
0.860
Alexithymia (TAS-20)
− 0.35
0.14
[− 0.59, − 0.07]
− 2.35
0.022*
Autism (AQ-10)
0.60
0.87
[− 1.45, 2.14]
0.54
0.461
Step three
     
Step three
     
Sex
− 0.45
0.34
[− 1.06, 0.26]
− 1.42
0.211
Sex
− 0.45
0.36
[− 1.11, 0.33]
− 1.42
0.212
Age (years)
3.60
2.83
[− 2.19, 9.27]
1.35
0.219
Age (years)
3.60
3.01
[− 2.71, 9.33]
1.35
0.257
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
0.80
3.45
[− 6.49, 7.39]
0.25
0.800
Anxiety (BSI-ANX)
0.80
3.31
[− 5.70, 7.34]
0.25
0.797
Depression (BSI-DEP)
1.04
5.03
[− 10.25, 9.96]
0.30
0.811
Depression (BSI-DEP)
1.04
4.97
[− 10.24, 9.5]
0.30
0.833
Alexithymia (TAS-20)
− 0.42
0.15
[− 0.68, − 0.11]
− 2.69
0.011*
Autism (AQ-10)
1.55
0.95
[− 0.55, 3.34]
1.39
0.088
Autism (AQ-10)
1.55
0.92
[− 0.46, 3.17]
1.39
0.090
Alexithymia (TAS)
− 0.42
0.15
[− 0.68, − 0.10]
− 2.69
0.009**
Model one: step one: R2 = 0.04, F(4, 62) = 0.59, p = 0.670, step two: ΔR2 = 0.08, ΔF(1, 61) = 5.50, p = 0.022*, step three: ΔR2 = 0.03, ΔF(1, 60) = 1.94, p = 0.169; model two: step one: R2 = 0.04, F(4, 62) = 0.59, p = 0.670, Step 2: ΔR2 = 0.00, ΔF(1, 61) = 0.29, p = 0.591, step three: ΔR2 = 0.10, ΔF(1, 60) = 7.21, p =0 .009**; SVO-A Social Value Orientation—Angle (Murphy et al., 2011), BSI-ANX Brief Symptom Inventory—Anxiety Scale (Derogatis, 2000), BSI-DEP Brief Symptom Inventory—Depression Scale (Derogatis, 2000), AQ-10 Autism Spectrum Quotient 10 (Allison et al., 2012; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), TAS-20 Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (Bagby et al., 1994a, 1994b; Parker et al., 2003)
*p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤0 .01

Discussion

To test whether alexithymic rather than autistic traits account for impairments in prosocial behavior, we administered a resource allocation task to a sample of healthy individuals whose autistic and alexithymic traits had been determined with personality questionnaires. Our well-powered and well-controlled analyses revealed the expected pattern of associations between task performance and personality traits: Individuals’ alexithymic traits were negatively associated with individuals’ prosocial behavior, indicating that individuals with high levels of alexithymia displayed less prosocial behavior than individuals with low levels of alexithymia. Individuals’ autistic traits, on the contrary, were neither positively nor negatively associated with individuals’ prosocial behavior, indicating that individuals with high levels of autism displayed as much prosocial behavior as individuals with low levels of autism. We, thus, assume that alexithymic rather than autistic traits impair prosocial behavior. To validate this assumption, our investigation has to be replicated with individuals who show a larger variability in alexithymic and autistic traits than our individuals. Investigations with autistic individuals and their first-degree relatives may be particularly useful for this purpose (Berthoz et al., 2013; Szatmari et al., 2008).
To understand why alexithymic rather than autistic traits impair prosocial behavior, it may be helpful to consider how alexithymia affects empathy in healthy and autistic individuals. Empathy, the ability to share and understand the emotions or thoughts of others, is a powerful motivator of prosocial behavior (Decety et al., 2016). Healthy individuals with high levels of empathy show more prosocial behavior than healthy individuals with low levels of empathy (Edele et al., 2013; Jordan et al., 2016), implying that alterations in empathy lead to profound alterations in prosocial behavior. Alexithymia alters empathy in healthy individuals (Grynberg et al., 2018). Healthy individuals with high levels of alexithymia are less able to share and understand the feelings of others than healthy individuals with low levels of alexithymia (Moriguchi et al., 2006, 2007; Parker et al., 2001). However, alexithymia also alters empathy in autistic individuals (Grynberg et al., 2018). Autistic individuals with high levels of alexithymia are also less able to share and understand the feelings of others than autistic individuals with low levels of alexithymia (Bird et al., 2010; Mul et al., 2018; Silani et al., 2008). We, thus, assume that alexithymia impairs prosocial behavior in healthy and autistic individuals by altering empathetic abilities that are relevant for the display of prosocial behavior (Decety et al., 2016). Although these assumptions appear to be somewhat speculative, we would like to point out that it has already been shown that alexithymia-dependent alterations of empathetic processes impair prosocial behavior among healthy individuals (Feldmanhall et al.,2013). Considering that autistic individuals display much higher levels of alexithymia and much lower levels of empathy than healthy individuals (Berthoz et al., 2013), we believe that alexithymia-dependent alterations of empathetic processes also contribute to impairments in prosocial behavior among autistic individuals.
We investigated how alexithymic and autistic traits impair prosocial aspects of social interaction, whereas others investigated how alexithymic and autistic traits impair emotional aspects of social cognition (Bird et al., 2010; Cook et al., 2013; Oakley et al., 2016; Silani et al., 2008). Although these investigations focused on impairments in different social domains, they nonetheless help to explain why some but not all autistic individuals show impairments in emotion recognition (Adolphs et al., 2001; Humphreys et al., 2007; Otsuka et al., 2017), empathetic responding (Dziobek et al., 2008; Hadjikhani et al., 2014; Rogers et al., 2007) and prosocial acting (Cage et al., , 2013; Ikuse et al., 2018; Izuma et al., 2011). Autistic individuals with high levels of alexithymia are more likely to display these and other impairments than autistic individuals with low levels of alexithymia (Bird & Cook, 2013). Given that the absence or presence of alexithymia has such profound effects on social functioning, we think that it is time to reconsider the current practice of diagnosing and treating ASD (Bird & Cook, 2013; Hobson et al., 2020). We believe that a thorough assessment of alexithymic and autistic traits facilitates the identification of individuals who benefit more from alexithymia-specific than autism-specific treatment approaches. We, therefore, hope that our investigation opens an avenue for novel approaches to the diagnosis and treatment of autistic individuals with different alexithymia levels.

Acknowledgments

AL was supported by a Grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG; LI 2517/2-1). The funding source had no further role in study design, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2), 232–240.PubMedCrossRef Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2), 232–240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Allison, C., Auyeung, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Toward brief “red flags” for autism screening: The short autism spectrum quotient and the short quantitative checklist for autism in toddlers in 1,000 cases and 3,000 controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(2), 202-212.e207.CrossRef Allison, C., Auyeung, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Toward brief “red flags” for autism screening: The short autism spectrum quotient and the short quantitative checklist for autism in toddlers in 1,000 cases and 3,000 controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(2), 202-212.e207.CrossRef
go back to reference APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23–32.PubMedCrossRef Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23–32.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 33–40.PubMedCrossRef Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 33–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berthoz, S., Lalanne, C., Crane, L., & Hill, E. L. (2013). Investigating emotional impairments in adults with autism-spectrum disorders and the broader autism phenotype. Psychiatry Research, 208(3), 257–264.PubMedCrossRef Berthoz, S., Lalanne, C., Crane, L., & Hill, E. L. (2013). Investigating emotional impairments in adults with autism-spectrum disorders and the broader autism phenotype. Psychiatry Research, 208(3), 257–264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133(Pt5), 1515–1525.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133(Pt5), 1515–1525.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Brewer, R., Marsh, A. A., Catmur, C., Cardinale, E. M., Stoycos, S., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2015). The impact of autism-spectrum disorder and alexithymia on judgments of moral acceptability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 589–595.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brewer, R., Marsh, A. A., Catmur, C., Cardinale, E. M., Stoycos, S., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2015). The impact of autism-spectrum disorder and alexithymia on judgments of moral acceptability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 589–595.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Cage, E., Pellicano, E., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Reputation management: Evidence for ability but reduced propensity in autism. Autism Research, 6(5), 433–442.PubMedCrossRef Cage, E., Pellicano, E., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Reputation management: Evidence for ability but reduced propensity in autism. Autism Research, 6(5), 433–442.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 24(5), 723–732.PubMedCrossRef Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 24(5), 723–732.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., Bartal, I. B., Uzefovsky, F., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2016). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150077.CrossRef Decety, J., Bartal, I. B., Uzefovsky, F., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2016). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150077.CrossRef
go back to reference Derogatis, L. R. (2000). BSI-18: Brief symptom inventory 18—administration scoring and procedures manual. NCS Pearson. Derogatis, L. R. (2000). BSI-18: Brief symptom inventory 18—administration scoring and procedures manual. NCS Pearson.
go back to reference Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 464–473.PubMedCrossRef Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 464–473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Edele, A., Dziobek, I., & Keller, M. (2013). Explaining altruistic sharing in the dictator game: The role of affective empathy, cognitive empathy, and justice sensitivity. Learning and Individual Differences, 24, 96–102.CrossRef Edele, A., Dziobek, I., & Keller, M. (2013). Explaining altruistic sharing in the dictator game: The role of affective empathy, cognitive empathy, and justice sensitivity. Learning and Individual Differences, 24, 96–102.CrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Feldmanhall, O., Dalgleish, T., & Mobbs, D. (2013). Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex, 49(3), 899–904.PubMedCrossRef Feldmanhall, O., Dalgleish, T., & Mobbs, D. (2013). Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex, 49(3), 899–904.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Franz, M., Popp, K., Schaefer, R., Sitte, W., Schneider, C., Hardt, J., Decker, O., & Braehler, E. (2008). Alexithymia in the German general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(1), 54–62.PubMedCrossRef Franz, M., Popp, K., Schaefer, R., Sitte, W., Schneider, C., Hardt, J., Decker, O., & Braehler, E. (2008). Alexithymia in the German general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(1), 54–62.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grynberg, D., Berthoz, S., & Bird, G. (2018). Social and interpersonal implications. Alexithymia: Advances in research, theory, and clinical practice (pp. 174–189). Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Grynberg, D., Berthoz, S., & Bird, G. (2018). Social and interpersonal implications. Alexithymia: Advances in research, theory, and clinical practice (pp. 174–189). Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Hadjikhani, N., Zürcher, N. R., Rogier, O., Hippolyte, L., Lemonnier, E., Ruest, T., Ward, N., Lassalle, A., Gillberg, N., Billstedt, E., & Helles, A. (2014). Emotional contagion for pain is intact in autism-spectrum disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 4, e343.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hadjikhani, N., Zürcher, N. R., Rogier, O., Hippolyte, L., Lemonnier, E., Ruest, T., Ward, N., Lassalle, A., Gillberg, N., Billstedt, E., & Helles, A. (2014). Emotional contagion for pain is intact in autism-spectrum disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 4, e343.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism-spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 290–322.PubMedCrossRef Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism-spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 290–322.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hendryx, M. S., Haviland, M. G., & Shaw, D. G. (1991). Dimensions of alexithymia and their relationships to anxiety and depression. Journal of Personality Assessment, 56(2), 227–237.PubMedCrossRef Hendryx, M. S., Haviland, M. G., & Shaw, D. G. (1991). Dimensions of alexithymia and their relationships to anxiety and depression. Journal of Personality Assessment, 56(2), 227–237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hobson, H., Westwood, H., Conway, J., McEwen, F. S., Colvert, E., Catmur, C., Bird, G., & Happe, F. (2020). Alexithymia and autism diagnostic assessments: Evidence from twins at genetic risk of autism and adults with anorexia nervosa. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 73, 101531.CrossRef Hobson, H., Westwood, H., Conway, J., McEwen, F. S., Colvert, E., Catmur, C., Bird, G., & Happe, F. (2020). Alexithymia and autism diagnostic assessments: Evidence from twins at genetic risk of autism and adults with anorexia nervosa. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 73, 101531.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Leonard, G. L., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A fine-grained analysis of facial expression processing in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 685–695.PubMedCrossRef Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Leonard, G. L., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A fine-grained analysis of facial expression processing in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 685–695.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ikuse, D., Tani, M., Itahashi, T., Yamada, H., Ohta, H., Morita, T., Arai, G., Saga, N., Tokumasu, T., Ohta, M., Sato, A., & Iwanami, A. (2018). The effect of visual cues on performance in the ultimatum game in individuals with autism-spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research, 259, 176–183.PubMedCrossRef Ikuse, D., Tani, M., Itahashi, T., Yamada, H., Ohta, H., Morita, T., Arai, G., Saga, N., Tokumasu, T., Ohta, M., Sato, A., & Iwanami, A. (2018). The effect of visual cues on performance in the ultimatum game in individuals with autism-spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research, 259, 176–183.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2011). Insensitivity to social reputation in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(42), 17302–17307.CrossRef Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2011). Insensitivity to social reputation in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(42), 17302–17307.CrossRef
go back to reference Jordan, M. R., Amir, D., & Bloom, P. (2016). Are empathy and concern psychologically distinct? Emotion, 16(8), 1107–1116.PubMedCrossRef Jordan, M. R., Amir, D., & Bloom, P. (2016). Are empathy and concern psychologically distinct? Emotion, 16(8), 1107–1116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kanai, C., Iwanami, A., Hashimoto, R., Ota, H., Tani, M., Yamada, T., & Kato, N. (2011). Clinical characterization of adults with Asperger’s syndrome assessed by self-report questionnaires based on depression, anxiety, and personality. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 5(4), 1451–1458.CrossRef Kanai, C., Iwanami, A., Hashimoto, R., Ota, H., Tani, M., Yamada, T., & Kato, N. (2011). Clinical characterization of adults with Asperger’s syndrome assessed by self-report questionnaires based on depression, anxiety, and personality. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 5(4), 1451–1458.CrossRef
go back to reference Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 505–521.CrossRef Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 505–521.CrossRef
go back to reference King-Casas, B., & Chiu, P. H. (2012). Understanding interpersonal function in psychiatric illness through multiplayer economic games. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 119–125.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef King-Casas, B., & Chiu, P. H. (2012). Understanding interpersonal function in psychiatric illness through multiplayer economic games. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 119–125.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kinnaird, E., Stewart, C., & Tchanturia, K. (2019). Investigating alexithymia in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Psychiatry, 55, 80–89.PubMedCrossRef Kinnaird, E., Stewart, C., & Tchanturia, K. (2019). Investigating alexithymia in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Psychiatry, 55, 80–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lischke, A., Mau-Moeller, A., Jacksteit, R., Pahnke, R., Hamm, A. O., & Weippert, M. (2018). Heart rate variability is associated with social value orientation in males but not females. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7336.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lischke, A., Mau-Moeller, A., Jacksteit, R., Pahnke, R., Hamm, A. O., & Weippert, M. (2018). Heart rate variability is associated with social value orientation in males but not females. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7336.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. (2007). Empathy and judging other’s pain: An fMRI study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 17(9), 2223–2234.PubMedCrossRef Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. (2007). Empathy and judging other’s pain: An fMRI study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 17(9), 2223–2234.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Lane, R. D., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. (2006). Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: An fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia. Neuroimage, 32(3), 1472–1482.PubMedCrossRef Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Lane, R. D., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. (2006). Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: An fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia. Neuroimage, 32(3), 1472–1482.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mul, C. L., Stagg, S. D., Herbelin, B., & Aspell, J. E. (2018). The feeling of me feeling for you: Interoception, alexithymia and empathy in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(9), 2953–2967.PubMedCrossRef Mul, C. L., Stagg, S. D., Herbelin, B., & Aspell, J. E. (2018). The feeling of me feeling for you: Interoception, alexithymia and empathy in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(9), 2953–2967.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Murphy, R. O., Ackermann, K. A., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 771–781.CrossRef Murphy, R. O., Ackermann, K. A., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 771–781.CrossRef
go back to reference Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A view of the psychosomatic process. In O. W. Hill (Ed.), Modern trends in psychosomatic medicine (Vol. 3, pp. 430–439). Butterworths. Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A view of the psychosomatic process. In O. W. Hill (Ed.), Modern trends in psychosomatic medicine (Vol. 3, pp. 430–439). Butterworths.
go back to reference Oakley, B. F. M., Brewer, R., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2016). Theory of mind is not theory of emotion: A cautionary note on the reading the mind in the eyes test. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(6), 818–823.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Oakley, B. F. M., Brewer, R., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2016). Theory of mind is not theory of emotion: A cautionary note on the reading the mind in the eyes test. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(6), 818–823.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Otsuka, S., Uono, S., Yoshimura, S., Zhao, S., & Toichi, M. (2017). Emotion perception mediates the predictive relationship between verbal ability and functional outcome in high-functioning adults with autism-spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(4), 1166–1182.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Otsuka, S., Uono, S., Yoshimura, S., Zhao, S., & Toichi, M. (2017). Emotion perception mediates the predictive relationship between verbal ability and functional outcome in high-functioning adults with autism-spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(4), 1166–1182.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2001). The relationship between emotional intelligence and alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(1), 107–115.CrossRef Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2001). The relationship between emotional intelligence and alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(1), 107–115.CrossRef
go back to reference Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2003). The 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. III. Reliability and factorial validity in a community population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55(3), 269–275.PubMedCrossRef Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2003). The 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. III. Reliability and factorial validity in a community population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55(3), 269–275.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 709–715.PubMedCrossRef Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 709–715.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Measuring autistic traits in the general population: A systematic review of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular autism, 6, 1–2. Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Watson, P., Auyeung, B., Ring, H., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Measuring autistic traits in the general population: A systematic review of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular autism, 6, 1–2.
go back to reference Silani, G., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2008). Levels of emotional awareness and autism: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 3(2), 97–112.PubMedCrossRef Silani, G., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2008). Levels of emotional awareness and autism: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 3(2), 97–112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Velikonja, T., Fett, A. K., & Velthorst, E. (2019). Patterns of nonsocial and social cognitive functioning in adults with autism-spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(2), 135–151.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Velikonja, T., Fett, A. K., & Velthorst, E. (2019). Patterns of nonsocial and social cognitive functioning in adults with autism-spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(2), 135–151.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wright, D. B., London, K., & Field, A. P. (2011). Using bootstrap estimation and the plug-in principle for clinical psychology data. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2(2), 252–270.CrossRef Wright, D. B., London, K., & Field, A. P. (2011). Using bootstrap estimation and the plug-in principle for clinical psychology data. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2(2), 252–270.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Alexithymic But Not Autistic Traits Impair Prosocial Behavior
Auteurs
Alexander Lischke
Harald J. Freyberger
Hans J. Grabe
Anett Mau-Moeller
Rike Pahnke
Publicatiedatum
28-06-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 6/2022
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05154-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2022

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 6/2022 Naar de uitgave