Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 19-10-2019 | Original Paper

Adaptation to the Speed of Biological Motion in Autism

Auteurs: Themis Karaminis, Roberto Arrighi, Georgia Forth, David Burr, Elizabeth Pellicano

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 2/2020

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Autistic individuals often present atypicalities in adaptation—the continuous recalibration of perceptual systems driven by recent sensory experiences. Here, we examined such atypicalities in human biological motion. We used a dual-task paradigm, including a running-speed discrimination task (‘comparing the speed of two running silhouettes’) and a change-detection task (‘detecting fixation-point shrinkages’) assessing attention. We tested 19 school-age autistic and 19 age- and ability-matched typical participants, also recording eye-movements. The two groups presented comparable speed-discrimination abilities and, unexpectedly, comparable adaptation. Accuracy in the change-detection task and the scatter of eye-fixations around the fixation point were also similar across groups. Yet, the scatter of fixations reliably predicted the magnitude of adaptation, demonstrating the importance of controlling for attention in adaptation studies.
Opmerkingen

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-019-04241-4) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Perceptual adaptation refers to the continuous recalibration of the response properties of perceptual and sensory systems driven by recent sensory experiences (Clifford and Rhodes 2005). For example, a quiet and continuous pure tone will be perceived to decrease in loudness over time (adaptation to loudness; see Lawson et al. 2015), while prolonged exposure to a face identity will cause a bias to perceive subsequently presented faces as dissimilar to it (adaptation to face identity; see Pellicano et al. 2007). Such adaptation is a ubiquitous property of perception and is thought to offer many functional advantages (e.g., Kohn 2007), in particular with regards to the efficiency with which sensory systems distinguish relevant from irrelevant stimuli. Limitations in adaptation should imply increases in the transmission of redundant information and should render individuals less able to distinguish relevant from irrelevant stimuli (Barlow 1990; Clifford et al. 2007; Webster et al. 2005). Such limitations could therefore have profound effects on how individuals perceive and interpret incoming sensory information.
Adaptation is also pertinent to theoretical accounts of autistic perception aiming to account for a range of sensory atypicalities and symptoms in the condition (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association 2013). Atypicalities in perceptual adaptation have been thought to reflect difficulties of autistic1 individuals in deriving or using prior knowledge representations accrued from recent sensory experiences (Pellicano and Burr 2012). Within the Bayesian inference, or predictive-coding theoretical frameworks, which, in broad terms, suggest that the brain continually exploits the statistics of the world to predict current sensory input using a hierarchical and bidirectional processing system which aims to minimise prediction error within a cascade of cortical processing (Clark 2013; Friston 2010), adaptation may relate to the atypical encoding of precision in the perceptual hierarchy in autism (Lawson et al. 2014) or the inability to process flexibly prediction errors (Van de Cruys et al. 2014).
Given the ubiquitous presence of adaptation in perception, an intriguing possibility is that autistic individuals’ atypicalities in adaptation are pervasive across perceptual domains. The presence of domain-general atypicalities in adaptation could account for sensory issues in autistic people (e.g., why they might find certain sounds particularly disturbing), as well as core social difficulties, on the basis of a common neural mechanism (Lawson et al. 2018).
With regard to social stimuli, attenuated adaptation in autism has been observed consistently within the face-processing domain, including, for example, for facial identity in autistic children (Ewing et al. 2013b; Pellicano et al. 2007) and relatives of autistic children (Fiorentini et al. 2012), for facial configuration (Ewing et al. 2013a, b) and eye-gaze direction in children (Pellicano et al. 2013) and adults (Lawson et al. 2018), and for emotional expressions in children (Rhodes et al. 2018) and adults (Rutherford et al. 2012). van Boxtel et al. (2016) also found that autistic children show reduced adaptation to action discrimination in biological motion (walking vs. running).
Turning to the processing of non-social stimuli, autistic children have been found to present attenuated adaptation to numerosity (Turi et al. 2015) and, in the auditory domain, autistic adults have been found to present attenuated adaptation to loudness (Lawson et al. 2015) and audiovisual integration (Turi et al. 2016). Three studies, however, have failed to find evidence of atypical adaptive-coding abilities, including Cook et al. (2014), who reported intact adaptation to facial expression and identity in autistic adults, Karaminis et al. (2015), who found that autistic and typical children did not differ in the degree of adaptation of perceptual causality, and Maule et al. (2018), who found that autistic and typical adults did not differ in the degree of adaptation to colour.
In this study, we contribute new evidence about the adaptive coding of the speed of biological motion in autistic children and adolescents. The examination of the adaptive coding of biological motion in autism is important for two reasons. First, the processing of biological motion is key for a wide range of social competencies, such as inferring other people’s emotions, mood, and intentions (e.g., Brooks et al. 2008). Previous research on the abilities of autistic individuals to process biological motion stimuli has produced mixed results. Autistic individuals have been found to present reduced sensitivity to biological motion and atypical brain activation patterns following the presentation of relevant biological stimuli in some studies (Annaz et al. 2012; Blake et al. 2003; Freitag et al. 2008; Klin and Jones 2008; Koldewyn et al. 2010; Nackaerts et al. 2012; Wang et al. 2015; see also Wang et al. 2018, for a recent behavioural genetics approach), but other studies have found no such difficulties (Cusack et al. 2015; Edey et al. 2019; Jones et al. 2011; Murphy et al. 2009; Saygin et al. 2010; van Boxtel et al. 2016). With regard to the adaptive coding of biological motion in autism, van Boxtel et al. (2016) found attenuated adaptation to action discrimination in autistic children while action discrimination (per se) was intact. There are (to our knowledge) no other studies examining the adaptive coding of biological motion in autism beyond action discrimination (van Boxtel et al. 2016).
Second, it is important to examine the adaptive coding of biological motion in autism to establish whether findings for attenuated adaptation in autism during the processing of social stimuli are specific to faces or extend to other, high-level social stimuli. This could be likely as biological motion is supported by high-level neuronal mechanisms within the superior temporal gyrus (STS) and the fusiform and the lingua gyri (Gobbini et al. 2007; Vaina et al. 2001), that is, brain areas that are also involved in the processing of faces (Grossman et al. 2000), as well as the extrastriate and fusiform body areas (EBA and FBA; Jastorff and Orban 2009).
In this study, we used a different paradigm for biological motion from that used in the study by van Boxtel et al. (2016). Our paradigm focuses on adaptive coding of the speed of running silhouettes presented with point light displays (PLDs). We employed child- and autism-friendly methodologies and we also aimed to account for participants’ attention to the stimuli. This was important as earlier studies have shown that attention modulates the size of adaptation (Kreutzer et al. 2015; Rhodes et al. 2011). Controlling for attention was achieved by employing a dual-task paradigm, in which the primary task measured the perception of biological motion and adaptive coding, while the secondary task motivated participants to attend to the middle of the screen and assessed their attention (see also Ewing et al. 2013b; Karaminis et al. 2015; Lawson et al. 2018; Rhodes et al. 2018). We also collected eye-movement data to quantify participants’ looking preferences during the task.

Method

Participants

Participants demographics are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for developmental variables for autistic and typical participants
Measures
Autistic participants
Typical participants
Statistical comparison
N
19
19
 
Gender (n females:n males)
6:13
11:8
X2(2, N = 38) = 1.72, p = 0.18
Age (years)
 Mean (SD)
14.15 (2.84)
13.93 (3.80)
t(36) = 0.23, p = 0.87
 Range
8.68–19.37
7.40–18.75
 
Verbal IQa
 Mean (SD)
104.68 (14.21)
105.47 (10.91)
t(36) = 0.19, p = 0.85
 Range
70–126
83–130
 
Performance IQa
 Mean (SD)
103.21 (18.94)
103.21 (18.95)
t(36) = 0.20, p = 0.85
 Range
75–132
76–139
 
Full-Scale IQa
 Mean (SD)
104.32 (16.57)
105.58 (12.65)
t(33.66) = 0.26, p = 0.79
 Range
80–132
77–138
 
ADOS-2 calibrated severity scoreb
 Mean (SD)
(N = 16) 4.75 (1.48)
n/a
n/a
 Range
3–7
  
SCQ score
 Mean (SD)
N = 17 21.24 (8.41)
N = 15 2.87 (3.35)
t(21.44) = 8.29, p < .001
 Range
5–37
0–12
 
SCQ Social Communication Questionnaire (score out of 40; Rutter et al. 2003)
aVerbal, Performance and Full-Scale IQ were measured using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence-2nd edition (WASI-II; 2011)
bADOS-2 calibrated severity scores obtained from Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 (Lord et al. 2012), scores range from 1 to 10, higher scores reflect greater autism severity

Autistic Participants

Nineteen autistic participants (6 girls) aged between 8.8 and 19.5 years (M = 14.15; SD = 2.84) were recruited via schools in London and community contacts. All autistic participants had an independent clinical diagnosis of an autism spectrum disorder and met the criteria for autism on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 (ADOS-2) (Lord et al. 2012; cut-off score = 7) or the Social Communication Questionnaire-Lifetime (SCQ; Rutter et al. 2003; cut-off score = 15) (see Corsello et al. 2007). All autistic participants were considered to be cognitively able, achieving scores ≥ 70 in the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence-2nd edition (WASI-II; 2011).

Typical Participants

Nineteen typically developing participants (10 girls), recruited from local London schools, were selected from a pool of 63 participants to match the group of autistic participants for chronological age, t(36) = 0.23, p = 0.87, gender, X2(2, N = 38) = 1.72, p = 0.18, as well as for performance IQ, t(36) = 0.20, p = 0.85; verbal IQ, t(36) = 0.19, p = 0.85; and full-scale IQ, t(33.66) = 0.26, p = 0.79, as measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence-2nd edition (WASI-II; 2011). Parents of typical participants also completed the SCQ (N = 11). SCQ scores of typical participants ranged between 0 and 12 (M = 2.64, SD = 3.50), below the cut-off point for autism (score of 15; Rutter et al. 2003).

Exclusions

Seven additional participants (3 autistic, 4 typical) were tested but excluded because of poorly-fitting psychometric curves, as judged by 2 observers who were blind to any demographic details of the participants (exclusion criterion #1). One additional typical child was excluded due to an IQ score lower than the threshold of 70 in the WASI-II (Wechsler 2011) (exclusion criterion #2). Five additional autistic and two additional typical participants were excluded due to poor performance on the attentional task (exclusion criterion #3, see “Measurements and Analysis” section). Finally, one additional autistic boy was excluded because he did not fixate centre-screen during the experimental task (exclusion criterion #4, see “Measurements and Analysis” section).

General Procedure and Ethics

The study was conducted in accordance to the principles laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. The UCL Institute of Education Research Ethics Committee approved all procedures. Parents of all participants gave their informed written consent prior to their child’s participation in the study and participants gave their verbal assent. Participants were tested individually in a quiet room at the Institute of Education. The WASI-II was administered on the same day, before or after the session. The ADOS-2 was administered either on the same day or on a separate occasion.

Stimuli and Apparatus

Adaptor and test stimuli (see Fig. 1; see also Arrighi et al. 2010) were PLDs comprising 10 dots of diameter 0.75° of visual angle and simulating running human figures. An original version of PLDs stimulus representing the running human silhouette was downloaded from an online database (http://​astro.​temple.​edu/​~tshipley/​ptltarchive.​html; Shipley 2012). This movie displayed a complete running cycle (starting with the left foot on the floor and ending with the left foot landing again) in 20 frames. Using customised interpolation scripts, we created 6000 points within each running cycle. We defined running speed as the number of running cycles completed within a second (in Hz).
Adaptor and test stimuli appeared on the left- or the right-hand side of the screen (centred 10° from the centre of the screen). The adaptor stimuli were two PLDs which appeared in grey colour and in pairs, simultaneously on the right- and left-hand side of the screen for 4.0 s. The adaptor stimuli fitted a 10° height × 5° width frame (‘medium-sized’) and moved at a speed of either 0.5 Hz or 2 Hz.
The test stimuli were two PLDs, the Reference stimulus and the Test stimulus (see Fig. 1). The Reference stimulus appeared in red colour on the left-hand side of the screen for 2.0 s. It fitted a 10° × 5° frame (‘medium-sized’) and moved at a speed of 1 Hz. The Test stimulus appeared in blue colour on the right-hand side of the screen for 2.0 s. It appeared in three possible sizes: small (within a frame of 8° height × 4° width), medium (10° × 5° frame), or large (12° × 6° frame) and at different speeds at the range 0.5–2 Hz.
For the change-detection task, the main stimulus was a round dot subtending 1.0° in the centre of the screen, which occasionally shrank to a diameter of 0.75° twice during each adaptation period.
All stimuli were displayed on a 60 Hz TFT monitor measuring 50° × 28° when viewed at a distance of 57 cm, controlled by a Dell Desktop computer. The experiments were written in MatLab using routines of the Psychophysics Toolbox 3 (Brainard 1997; Pelli 1997; Kleiner et al. 2007). Eye-tracking data were collected using a Tobii-X300 eye tracker at 120 Hz and were processed with the Tobii Analytics Software Development Kit (SDK).

Procedure

We measured perceptual adaptation to the speed of biological motion using a developmentally-sensitive computer game, which combined a speed-discrimination task, assessing adaptation to the speed of biological motion, and a change-detection task, motivating participants to attend to the centre of the screen. The general theme of the game was that participants were ‘Space Running Trainers’ aiming to form a winning team for the ‘Space Olympics’. To do so, participants should choose the fastest runners using a ‘specialised viewing machine’ (which provided the PLDs). The task structure and the trial structure are presented in Fig. 1.

Speed-Discrimination Task

The speed-discrimination task comprised two conditions, Right and Left (‘rounds’, counterbalanced across participants), each consisting of 40 trials presented in blocks (‘Levels’) of 13, 13, and 14 trials. Each trial included an adaptation phase, in which participants were exposed to adaptor stimuli, followed by a testing phase, in which participants judged the speed of test stimuli. The adaptation phase was differentiated in the Right and the Left condition so as to elicit adaptation aftereffects in two opposite directions (see also “Measurements and Analysis” section). The two conditions of the speed-discrimination task thus implemented a so-called ‘push–pull’ adaptation protocol.
In the adaptation phase, which lasted 4.0 s, participants watched the adaptor PLDs while they were encouraged to attend to the fixation point centre-screen (see also “Change-Detection Task” section). In the Right condition, the speed of the right adaptor PLD was 2 Hz, four times faster than the left adaptor (0.5 Hz). Conversely, in the Left condition, the right adaptor that ran at 0.5 Hz and the left at 2 Hz.
In the test phase, participants were presented with the two test PLDs, first the Reference stimulus on the left-hand-side of the screen and then the Test stimulus on the right-hand-side of the screen, for 2.0 s each. They were asked to indicate which runner they thought was the fastest by pressing a corresponding red or blue key on the keyboard. Responses were not registered until both runners had finished running.
The speed of the Reference PLD always was set at 1.0 Hz. The speed of the Test PLD was chosen using two QUEST functions (Watson and Pelli 1983), one starting at 0.5 Hz and ascending and one starting at 2.0 Hz and descending. The two QUESTs homed in on the point where the speed of the two test stimuli appeared equal; to ensure a good distribution of durations to estimate discrimination thresholds, a random jitter of SD = 0.1 log units was also added to the QUEST estimates (Watson and Pelli 1983).
The Test stimulus appeared in three possible sizes, small (8° × 4°), medium (10° × 5°), and large (12° × 6°). This manipulation ensured that our participants could not solve the discrimination task by relying on the local speed of the dots constituting the PLDs (see also van Boxtel and Lu 2013). For example, let’s assume that the two test stimuli (Reference and Test) moved at the same speed (say, a gait cycle per second) and that the Test stimulus was small. Because of this size difference, the distance covered by the individual dots of the Test stimulus (e.g., the feet) during a cycle gait would be shorter than the distance covered by the corresponding dots of the Reference stimulus. Based on this difference, if participants relied on a local-speed response strategy, they should present a bias to respond that the Reference stimulus would be faster. By contrast, if participants relied on a global response strategy, they should not present this bias.

Change-Detection Task

In the change-detection task, participants were asked to respond to changes of the fixation (‘viewing machine losing power’) point by pressing the spacebar (‘powering up the machine’). The fixation point returned to normal after a response. The change-detection task took place during the adaptation phase of the trials of the speed discrimination task. There were zero, one or two shrinkage events in each trial, each lasting 1 s.

Practice Trials and Motivation

Participants were given visual and verbal instructions for both tasks at the start of the game, including practice on pressing the spacebar when the dot in the centre of the screen shrank. They also completed eight practice trials, in which the speed of each of the running figures in the testing phase were very clearly different from each other (0.5 Hz vs. 1.5 Hz or 2.0 Hz). Practice trials were repeated if participants made more than three mistakes or if they responded that they needed more practice to proceed to the actual game. This happened only for two autistic participants and never more than once. Participants had the opportunity to take short breaks at the end of the testing blocks. They were regularly praised for their performance and, at the end of each round, they were shown a leaderboard. The experimenter encouraged them to attend to the centre of the screen throughout testing and monitored their attention.

Measurements and Analysis

Speed-Discrimination Task

Figure 2 shows example data from two of our participants from the speed-discrimination task. We fitted individual data from participants with cumulative Gaussian functions using bootstrapping (Efron and Tibshirani 1993) with 10 repetitions and a ‘maximum likelihood’ fitting method (Watson 1981). First, two observers, blind to any demographic details, judged the quality of the fitted curves. Participants with poorly fitting curves were excluded from the analysis. From the fitted curves, and for each condition, we derived Weber Fractions [the standard deviations of the fitted Gaussians or just noticeable difference (JND) divided by the Points of Subjective Equality (PSE)] and the PSEs (the mean of the fitted Gaussians).
Weber Fractions provided an estimate of the precision with which participants judged the speed of the PLDs. We compared Weber Fractions using a repeated-measures ANOVA with Condition (‘Left’ vs. ‘Right’) as a between-participants factor and Group (‘Autistic’ vs. ‘Typical’).
The PSE corresponded to the value of the Test stimulus intensity (more precisely, the value of the log-transformed ratio speed of Test stimulus: speed of Reference stimulus) for which the judgements of participants in the speed-discrimination task were at chance levels, that is, participants responded that the Test PLD was faster than the Reference PLD with a probability of 0.5. For each participant, we derived PSE_Right and the PSE_Left using data from the Right and the Left condition, correspondingly. In our data, due to adaptation, PSE_Right tended to be higher than PSE_Left. This was as in the Left (Right) condition, the Test stimulus was presented after exposure to a slow (fast) adaptor and was thus perceived to be faster (slower), pushing (pulling) the psychometric curve to the left (right) (see Fig. 2). To estimate the magnitude of the adaptation effect we calculated the distance PSE_Right − PSE_Left. We compared the magnitude of adaptation in the two matched groups with an independent samples t-tests. We also performed a complementary Bayesian independent samples t test for this difference.

Change-Detection Task

For the change-detection task, we calculated mean accuracy (the proportion of detected shrinkages) in the change-detection task across both conditions. Participants with accuracy scores lower than 25% were excluded from the analysis. We also examined reaction times in the change-detection task (online measure).

Eye-Tracking Data

From the eye tracking data, we calculated the scatter of fixations around the centre of the screen (the standard deviation of average distance from the centre of the screen) during the adaptation and the testing phase. One autistic participant, with a scatter of fixation of 15.0° of the visual angle was excluded from the analysis. We also calculated correlations between the scatter of fixations and adaptation in the speed-discrimination task.

Correlational Analysis

In a secondary analysis, we examined correlations between adaptation to the speed of biological motion and precision in speed discrimination, as well as correlations between adaptation and demographic and eye-tracking variables.

Results

Similar Speed-Discrimination Precision and Similar Adaptation to the Speed of Biological Motion

First, we looked at precision in discriminating the speed of biological motion, expressed as Weber Fractions. Figure 3 shows Weber Fractions in the two conditions of the speed discrimination tasks (Left, autistic: M = 0.40, SD = 0.23; typical: M = 0.37, SD = 0.22; Right, autistic: M = 0.42, SD = 0.36; typical: M = 0.37, SD = 0.18). We conducted a mixed-design ANOVA with Group (‘Autistic’ vs. ‘Typical’) as a between-participants factor and Condition (‘Left’ vs. ‘Right’) as a within-participants factor. There were no significant effects of Group, F(1, 36) = 0.39, p = 0.54, n p 2  = 0.01; Condition, F(1, 36) = 0.01, p = 0.93, n p 2  < 0.01; and no significant interaction between the two factors, F(1, 36) = 0.04, p = 0.84, n p 2  = 0.001. Our analysis therefore suggested that autistic and typical participants presented similar precision in speed-discrimination.
Next, we examined the magnitude of adaptation, shown in Fig. 4 (autistic participants: M = 0.60, SD = 0.20; typical participants: M = 0.55, SD = 0.26). The magnitude of the adaptation effect was significantly higher than 0 in both groups of participants, as revealed by one-sample t-test [autistic participants: t(18) = 13.36, p < 0.001; typical participants: t(18) = 10.77, p < 0.001]. Importantly, and contrary to our prediction, there were no differences in adaptation between autistic and typical participants, t(36) = 0.50, p = 0.48, d = 0.20.
We also performed a Bayesian independent samples t-test using JASP software (Version 0.8.0.0; JASP Team 2016) and estimated a Bayes factor using Bayesian information criteria (Wagenmakers 2007), which allowed for a comparison of the fit of our data under the null hypothesis that there are no differences between autistic and typical children in the magnitude of the adaptation to the speed of biological motion, and the alternative hypothesis that adaptation differs in the two groups of participants. The Bayes factor (null/alternative-estimated using a Cauchy distribution prior with a scaling factor of 1) was 3.38, suggesting that our results were 3.38 times more likely to occur under the null hypothesis than under the alternative hypothesis. Our data, therefore, provided substantial evidence (Wetzels et al. 2011) that autistic and typical participants adapted to the speed of biological motion to a comparable degree.

Similar Performance in the Change-Detection Task

Turning to the change-detection task, Fig. 5 shows accuracy rates in the two conditions of the task (Left, autistic: M = 0.78, SD = 0.17; typical: M = 0.79, SD = 0.17; Right, autistic: M = 0.75, SD = 0.22; typical: M = 0.72, SD = 0.21). A mixed-design ANOVA with Group (‘Autistic’ vs. ‘Typical’) as a between-participants factor and Condition (‘Left’ vs. ‘Right’) as a within-participants factor showed no effects of Group, F(1, 36) = 0.27, p = 0.87, n p 2 = 0.001, a significant effect of Condition, F(1, 36) = 6.16, p = 0.02, n p 2 = 0.15, and no significant interaction between Condition and Group, F(1, 36) = 0.99, p = 0.32, n p 2 = 0.00. Autistic and typical participants performed similarly on the secondary task.

Similar Reaction Times in the Change-Detection Task

For the change-detection task, we examined mean reaction times, shown in Fig. 6 (Left, autistic: M = 2.84, SD = 0.94; typical: M = 2.74, SD = 0.83; Right, autistic: M = 2.74, SD = 0.83; typical: M = 2.47, SD = 0.52). A mixed-design ANOVA with Group (‘Autistic’ vs. ‘Typical’) as the between-participants factor and Condition (‘Left’ vs. ‘Right’) as the within-participants factor showed no significant effects of Group, F(1, 36) = 2.40, p = 0.13, n p 2  = 0.06, or Condition, F(1, 36) = 0.12, p = 0.73, n p 2  = 0.003, or condition × group interaction, F(1, 36) = 0.36, p = 0.54, n p 2 = 0.01. The results therefore suggested that autistic and typical participants did not differ in their reaction times.

Similar Eye-Movement Data

We also examined eye-tracking data to obtain an objective measure of the extent to which participants attended to the centre of the screen (as motivated by the change-detection task, as well as by the experimenter during the testing session). Figure 7 shows the scatter of fixations around centre-screen in the two conditions (in degrees of the visual angle) (Left, autistic: M = 0.035, SD = 0.014; typical: M = 0.039, SD = 0.025; Right, autistic: M = 0.038, SD = 0.020; typical: M = 0.044, SD = 0.032). Again, a mixed-design ANOVA with Group (‘Autistic’ vs. ‘Typical’) as a between-participants factor and Condition (‘Left’ vs. ‘Right’) as a within-participants factor and showed no significant effects of Group, F(1, 36) = 0.54, p = 0.47, n p 2  = 0.02, Condition, F(1, 36) = 1.08, p = 0.31, n p 2  = 0.03, and no significant interaction, F(1, 36) = 0.11, p = 0.74, n p 2  = 0.00. Autistic and typical participants fixated to centre-screen to a comparable extent.

Correlational Analysis

In a secondary correlational analysis, we examined the relationship between adaptation to the speed of biological motion and precision in speed discrimination, as well as between and adaptation demographic and eye-tracking variables (Fig. 8). Correlations between adaptation to the speed of biological motion and precision were non-significant in either group of participants [autistic: r(19) = − 0.17, p = 0.55; typical: r(19) = − 0.36, p = 0.13]. Furthermore, in either group of participants, there were no significant correlations between adaptation and age [autistic: r (19) = 0.08, p = 0.78; typical: r(19) = 0.04, p = 0.99], and Performance-IQ [autistic: r (19) = 0.06, p = 0.79; typical: r(19) = 0.22, p = 0.36] and Verbal-IQ [autistic: r(19) = 0.36, p = 0.13; typical: r(19) = − 0.18, p = 0.46]. Within the group autistic participants, there were also no significant correlations between the magnitude of adaptation and autistic features, as indexed by ADOS-2 calibrated severity scores, r(16) = − 0.35, p = 0.18, or SCQ scores, r(17) = 0.23, p = 0.38. Correlations between the magnitude of adaptation and SCQ scores were also not significant when autistic and typical participants were considered as one group, r(28) = 0.08, p = 0.66.
Interestingly, there was a significant correlation between the magnitude of adaptation and the eye-tracking variable of the scatter of fixations in both autistic, r(19) = − 0.62, p = 0.005, and typical participants, r(19) = − 0.61, p = 0.01. As shown in Fig. 8h, the adaptation effect is less pronounced for participants who attended to a lesser extent to centre-screen. Note that correlations between the eye-movement measure and precision in speed-discrimination [autistic: r(19) = 0.15, p = 0.54; typical: r(19) = 0.34, p = 0.16] were non-significant.

Discussion

In this study, we compared autistic and typical participants, of similar age and ability, on the adaptive coding of the speed of biological motion. We hypothesised that autistic individuals’ atypicalities in the adaptive coding of facial stimuli (Ewing et al. 2013a; Lawson et al. 2018; Pellicano et al. 2013; Rhodes et al. 2018; Rutherford et al. 2012) should generalise to non-facial social stimuli and predicted that autistic participants should show less adaptation to the speed of the PLDs of our task than the typical comparison participants. We found that both groups showed significant adaptation effects—but, contrary to our prediction, that the magnitude of adaptation was comparable in autistic and typical participants. This finding could not be attributed to group differences in attention or to looking differences, as both accuracy on the change-detection task and the scatter-of-fixations measure were similar across groups.
Furthermore, the lack of differences in adaptation between autistic and typical participants could not be due to differences in precision in speed discrimination. We found that the two groups were equally precise. This latter result is consistent with studies that do not find differences in the processing of biological motion in autism (Cusack et al. 2015; Jones et al. 2011; Murphy et al. 2009; Saygin et al. 2010; van Boxtel et al. 2016) rather than those that report reduced sensitivity and differences in the brain activation patterns to biological stimuli (Annaz et al. 2012; Blake et al. 2003; Freitag et al. 2008; Klin and Jones 2008; Koldewyn et al. 2010; Nackaerts et al. 2012).
Our results are also inconsistent with the study on adaptation to biological motion by van Boxtel et al. (2016), which examined a similar number of autistic and typical children. It is possible that this discrepancy is due to the focus on different aspects of biological motion (“running speed” vs. discrimination of type of movement in van Boxtel et al. 2016). It is difficult to understand the origin of these discrepancies without further investigation of performance in different types of biological motion within the same individual. It would be interesting to replicate our and van Boxtel et al.’s methods, also considering other biological motion characteristics such as gender, which is more explicitly social and to which adaptation has previously been shown in non-autistic adults (Jordan et al. 2006; Troje et al. 2006).
Another factor that could be considered in future studies is the likely correspondence between the kinematics of the test stimuli and the kinematics of participants. One study has reported that autistic adults present atypical kinematics and that the degree of such atypicalities predicts performance in a biological motion perception task (Cook et al. 2013). It is possible that the perceptual similarity or dissimilarity between the kinematics of stimuli and participants could also affect the adaptive coding of biological motion.
One important methodological feature of our study is that it carefully examined differences in attention. This was achieved by including the secondary change-detection task and using eye-tracking. By contrast, in van Boxtel et al. (2016), where autistic children were found to present attenuated adaptation, “the experimenter monitored fixation throughout the experiment, providing reminders as deemed necessary” (p. 4). Arguably, the use of a change-detection task is a more robust method for directing participants’ attention to the fixation point. Interestingly, the post hoc analysis of the eye-tracking data showed that the more participants attended to the fixation point, the larger the magnitude of adaptation. Therefore, even though autistic participants did not differ on average from typical participants on the degree of adaptation, the scatter of fixation accounted for adaptation performance. This result raises the possibility that differences in adaptation in many studies could result from attention differences. It is thus also very important to control for attention in adaptation studies (see also gaze-contingent paradigms; e.g., Wilms et al. 2010). To our knowledge, controlling for attention has been employed in earlier studies on adaptation in autism by Ewing et al. (2013b) on face identity, Karaminis et al. (2015) on perceptual causality, Lawson et al. (2018) on eye-gaze direction and Rhodes et al. (2018) on facial expression. Our study on adaptation to the running speed of biological motion in autism is novel in combining the use of a secondary attention task with eye-tracking.
Our study is not without its shortcomings. We applied four exclusion criteria and thus excluded a considerable number of participants from our initial dataset to obtain a dataset that would allow measuring the adaptive coding of biological motion. The dual-task paradigm was also demanding, especially for younger participants. Finally, adaptation to biological motion in participants who were not able to attend to stimuli was also not explored in this study.

Conclusion

Sensory differences have been included in the latest diagnostic criteria for autism (DSM-5; APA 2013) and represent some of the most puzzling features of the condition. The renewed interest in autistic sensory differences by researchers is prompted largely by the possibility that these and other non-social features of autism might be caused by fundamental differences in sensation and perception. Our results provide evidence that diminished adaptation, proposed to be one such fundamental difference, is not pervasive in autistic perception. Our findings demonstrate that more nuanced accounts of adaptation in autism are warranted, which address the potentially uneven adaptation profile in autism and its developmental implications (cf. Karaminis et al. 2015). The interplay between adaptation and attention is also important for a fuller understanding of autistic perception.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to the young participants, families and school staff who kindly took part in this research. Thanks also to Lorcan Kenny, Katy Warren, and Hannah White for their help in collecting the data.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest of which they are aware.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in the study involving human participants were in accordance with the Ethical Standards of the Institutional and/or National Research Committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Parents of all child/adolescent participants gave their informed written consent prior to their child’s participation in the study and young participants gave their informed verbal assent.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Voetnoten
1
We use ‘identify-first’ language (‘autistic person’) rather than person-first language (‘person with autism’), because it is the preferred term of autistic activists (e.g., Sinclair 1999) and many autistic people and their families (Kenny et al. 2016) and is less associated with stigma (Gernsbacher 2017).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRef
go back to reference Annaz, D., Campbell, R., Coleman, M., Milne, E., & Swettenham, J. (2012). Young children with autism spectrum disorder do not preferentially attend to biological motion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,42(3), 401–408.CrossRefPubMed Annaz, D., Campbell, R., Coleman, M., Milne, E., & Swettenham, J. (2012). Young children with autism spectrum disorder do not preferentially attend to biological motion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,42(3), 401–408.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Arrighi, R., Orsi, S., & Burr, D. C. (2010). Adaptation of speed and duration of biological motion stimuli. Perception,39(ECVP Abstract Supplement), 151. Arrighi, R., Orsi, S., & Burr, D. C. (2010). Adaptation of speed and duration of biological motion stimuli. Perception,39(ECVP Abstract Supplement), 151.
go back to reference Barlow, H. (1990). Conditions for versatile learning, Helmholtz’s unconscious inference, and the task of perception. Vision Research,30, 1561–1571.CrossRefPubMed Barlow, H. (1990). Conditions for versatile learning, Helmholtz’s unconscious inference, and the task of perception. Vision Research,30, 1561–1571.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science,14(2), 151–157.CrossRefPubMed Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science,14(2), 151–157.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brooks, A., Schouten, B., Troje, N. F., Verfaillie, K., Blanke, O., & van der Zwan, R. (2008). Correlated changes in perceptions of the gender and orientation of ambiguous biological motion figures. Current Biology,18, 728–729.CrossRef Brooks, A., Schouten, B., Troje, N. F., Verfaillie, K., Blanke, O., & van der Zwan, R. (2008). Correlated changes in perceptions of the gender and orientation of ambiguous biological motion figures. Current Biology,18, 728–729.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,36, 181–204.CrossRef Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,36, 181–204.CrossRef
go back to reference Clifford, C. W. G., & Rhodes, G. (2005). Fitting the mind to the world: Introduction. In C. W. Clifford & G. Rhodes (Eds.), Fitting the mind to the world: Adaptation and after-effects in high-level vision (Vol. 2, pp. 1–12). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Clifford, C. W. G., & Rhodes, G. (2005). Fitting the mind to the world: Introduction. In C. W. Clifford & G. Rhodes (Eds.), Fitting the mind to the world: Adaptation and after-effects in high-level vision (Vol. 2, pp. 1–12). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Clifford, C. W. G., Webster, M. A., Stanley, G. B., Stocker, A. A., Kohn, A., Sharpee, T. O., et al. (2007). Visual adaptation: Neural, psychological and computational aspects. Vision Research,47, 3125–3131.CrossRefPubMed Clifford, C. W. G., Webster, M. A., Stanley, G. B., Stocker, A. A., Kohn, A., Sharpee, T. O., et al. (2007). Visual adaptation: Neural, psychological and computational aspects. Vision Research,47, 3125–3131.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2014). Intact facial adaptation in autistic adults. Autism Research,7(4), 481–490.CrossRefPubMed Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2014). Intact facial adaptation in autistic adults. Autism Research,7(4), 481–490.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Corsello, C., Hus, V., Pickles, A., Risi, S., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., et al. (2007). Between a ROC and a hard place: Decision making and making decisions about using the SCQ. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,48, 932–940.CrossRefPubMed Corsello, C., Hus, V., Pickles, A., Risi, S., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., et al. (2007). Between a ROC and a hard place: Decision making and making decisions about using the SCQ. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,48, 932–940.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cusack, J. P., Williams, J. H., & Neri, P. (2015). Action perception is intact in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neuroscience,35(5), 1849–1857.CrossRefPubMed Cusack, J. P., Williams, J. H., & Neri, P. (2015). Action perception is intact in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neuroscience,35(5), 1849–1857.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Edey, R., Cook, J., Brewer, R., Bird, G., & Press, C. (2019). Adults with autism spectrum disorder are sensitive to the kinematic features defining natural human motion. Autism Research,12, 284–294.CrossRefPubMed Edey, R., Cook, J., Brewer, R., Bird, G., & Press, C. (2019). Adults with autism spectrum disorder are sensitive to the kinematic features defining natural human motion. Autism Research,12, 284–294.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Efron, B., & Tibshirani, T. J. (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap. London: Chapman and Hall.CrossRef Efron, B., & Tibshirani, T. J. (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap. London: Chapman and Hall.CrossRef
go back to reference Ewing, L., Leach, K., Pellicano, E., Jeffery, L., & Rhodes, G. (2013a). Reduced face aftereffects in autism are not due to poor attention. PLoS ONE,8(11), e81353.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ewing, L., Leach, K., Pellicano, E., Jeffery, L., & Rhodes, G. (2013a). Reduced face aftereffects in autism are not due to poor attention. PLoS ONE,8(11), e81353.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Ewing, L., Pellicano, E., & Rhodes, G. (2013b). Atypical updating of face representations with experience in children with autism. Developmental Science,16(1), 116–123.CrossRefPubMed Ewing, L., Pellicano, E., & Rhodes, G. (2013b). Atypical updating of face representations with experience in children with autism. Developmental Science,16(1), 116–123.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fiorentini, C., Gray, L., Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., & Pellicano, E. (2012). Reduced face identity aftereffects in relatives of children with autism. Neuropsychologia,50(12), 2926–2932.CrossRefPubMed Fiorentini, C., Gray, L., Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., & Pellicano, E. (2012). Reduced face identity aftereffects in relatives of children with autism. Neuropsychologia,50(12), 2926–2932.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Häberlein, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., et al. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia,46, 1480–1494.CrossRefPubMed Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Häberlein, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., et al. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia,46, 1480–1494.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Friston, K. J. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Neuroscience,11, 127–138.CrossRef Friston, K. J. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Neuroscience,11, 127–138.CrossRef
go back to reference Gernsbacher, M. A. (2017). The use of person-first language in scholarly writing may accentuate sigma. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,58, 859–861.CrossRefPubMed Gernsbacher, M. A. (2017). The use of person-first language in scholarly writing may accentuate sigma. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,58, 859–861.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gobbini, M. I., Koralek, A. C., Bryan, R. E., Montgomery, K. J., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory of mind tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,19, 1803–1814.CrossRefPubMed Gobbini, M. I., Koralek, A. C., Bryan, R. E., Montgomery, K. J., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory of mind tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,19, 1803–1814.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Morgan, V., Pickens, D., Neighbor, G., et al. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,12, 711–720.CrossRefPubMed Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Morgan, V., Pickens, D., Neighbor, G., et al. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,12, 711–720.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jastorff, J., & Orban, G. A. (2009). Human fMRI reveals separation and integration of shape and motion cues in biological motion processing. Journal of Neuroscience,29, 7315–7329.CrossRefPubMed Jastorff, J., & Orban, G. A. (2009). Human fMRI reveals separation and integration of shape and motion cues in biological motion processing. Journal of Neuroscience,29, 7315–7329.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research,4(5), 347–357.CrossRefPubMed Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research,4(5), 347–357.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jordan, H., Fallah, M., & Stoner, G. R. (2006). Adaptation of gender derived from biological motion. Nature Neuroscience,9, 738–739.CrossRefPubMed Jordan, H., Fallah, M., & Stoner, G. R. (2006). Adaptation of gender derived from biological motion. Nature Neuroscience,9, 738–739.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Karaminis, T., Turi, M., Neil, L., Badcock, N. A., Burr, D., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Atypicalities in perceptual adaptation in autism do not extend to perceptual causality. PLoS ONE,10(3), e0120439.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Karaminis, T., Turi, M., Neil, L., Badcock, N. A., Burr, D., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Atypicalities in perceptual adaptation in autism do not extend to perceptual causality. PLoS ONE,10(3), e0120439.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). What terms should we use to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism,20, 442–462.CrossRefPubMed Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). What terms should we use to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism,20, 442–462.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., Pelli, D., Ingling, A., Murray, R., & Broussard, C. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception,36(14), 1–46. Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., Pelli, D., Ingling, A., Murray, R., & Broussard, C. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception,36(14), 1–46.
go back to reference Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2008). Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism. Developmental Science,11(1), 40–46.CrossRefPubMed Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2008). Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism. Developmental Science,11(1), 40–46.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kohn, A. (2007). Visual adaptation: Physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. Journal of Neurophysiology,97, 3155–3164.CrossRefPubMed Kohn, A. (2007). Visual adaptation: Physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. Journal of Neurophysiology,97, 3155–3164.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. M. (2010). The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. Brain,133, 599–610.CrossRefPubMed Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. M. (2010). The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. Brain,133, 599–610.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kreutzer, S., Fink, G. R., & Weidner, R. (2015). Attention modulates visual size adaptation. Journal of Vision,15, 1–9.CrossRef Kreutzer, S., Fink, G. R., & Weidner, R. (2015). Attention modulates visual size adaptation. Journal of Vision,15, 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Lawson, R. P., Aylward, J., Roiser, J. P., & Rees, G. (2018). Adaptation of social and non-social cues to direction in adults with autism spectrum disorder and neurotypical adults with autistic traits. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,29, 108–116.CrossRefPubMed Lawson, R. P., Aylward, J., Roiser, J. P., & Rees, G. (2018). Adaptation of social and non-social cues to direction in adults with autism spectrum disorder and neurotypical adults with autistic traits. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,29, 108–116.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lawson, R. P., Aylward, J., White, S., & Rees, G. (2015). A striking reduction of loudness adaptation in autism. Nature Scientific Reports,5, 16157.CrossRef Lawson, R. P., Aylward, J., White, S., & Rees, G. (2015). A striking reduction of loudness adaptation in autism. Nature Scientific Reports,5, 16157.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Maule, J., Stanworth, K., Pellicano, E., & Franklin, A. (2018). Color afterimages in autistic adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,48(4), 1409–1421.CrossRefPubMed Maule, J., Stanworth, K., Pellicano, E., & Franklin, A. (2018). Color afterimages in autistic adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,48(4), 1409–1421.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., & Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia,47(14), 3225–3235.CrossRefPubMed Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., & Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia,47(14), 3225–3235.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nackaerts, E., Wagemans, J., Helsen, W., Swinnen, S. P., Wenderoth, N., & Alaerts, K. (2012). Recognizing biological motion and emotions from point-light displays in autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE,7(9), e44473.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Nackaerts, E., Wagemans, J., Helsen, W., Swinnen, S. P., Wenderoth, N., & Alaerts, K. (2012). Recognizing biological motion and emotions from point-light displays in autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE,7(9), e44473.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision,10(4), 437–442.CrossRefPubMed Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision,10(4), 437–442.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes too real: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,16, 504–510.CrossRefPubMed Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes too real: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,16, 504–510.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pellicano, E., Jeffery, L., Burr, D., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Abnormal adaptive face-coding mechanisms in children with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology,17, 1508–1515.CrossRefPubMed Pellicano, E., Jeffery, L., Burr, D., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Abnormal adaptive face-coding mechanisms in children with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology,17, 1508–1515.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pellicano, E., Rhodes, G., & Calder, A. J. (2013). Reduced gaze aftereffects are related to difficulties categorising gaze direction in children with autism. Neuropsychologia,51(8), 1504–1509.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pellicano, E., Rhodes, G., & Calder, A. J. (2013). Reduced gaze aftereffects are related to difficulties categorising gaze direction in children with autism. Neuropsychologia,51(8), 1504–1509.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rhodes, G., Burton, N., Jeffery, L., Read, A., Taylor, L., & Ewing, L. (2018). Facial expression coding in children and adolescents with autism: Reduced adaptability but intact norm-based coding. British Journal of Psychology,109, 204–218.CrossRefPubMed Rhodes, G., Burton, N., Jeffery, L., Read, A., Taylor, L., & Ewing, L. (2018). Facial expression coding in children and adolescents with autism: Reduced adaptability but intact norm-based coding. British Journal of Psychology,109, 204–218.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Evangelista, E., Ewing, L., Peters, M., et al. (2011). Enhanced attention amplifies face adaptation. Vision Research,51, 1811–1819.CrossRefPubMed Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Evangelista, E., Ewing, L., Peters, M., et al. (2011). Enhanced attention amplifies face adaptation. Vision Research,51, 1811–1819.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rutherford, M. D., Troubridge, E. K., & Walsh, J. (2012). Visual afterimages of emotional faces in high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,42(2), 221–229.CrossRefPubMed Rutherford, M. D., Troubridge, E. K., & Walsh, J. (2012). Visual afterimages of emotional faces in high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,42(2), 221–229.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Saygin, A. P., Cook, J., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE,5(10), e13491.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Saygin, A. P., Cook, J., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE,5(10), e13491.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Troje, N. F., Sadr, J., Geyer, H., & Nakayama, K. (2006). Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of gender from biological motion. Journal of Vision,6(8), 7.CrossRef Troje, N. F., Sadr, J., Geyer, H., & Nakayama, K. (2006). Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of gender from biological motion. Journal of Vision,6(8), 7.CrossRef
go back to reference Turi, M., Burr, D. C., Igliozzi, R., Aagten-Murphy, D., Muratori, F., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorder show reduced adaptation to number. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA,112(25), 7868–7872.CrossRef Turi, M., Burr, D. C., Igliozzi, R., Aagten-Murphy, D., Muratori, F., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorder show reduced adaptation to number. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA,112(25), 7868–7872.CrossRef
go back to reference Turi, M., Karaminis, T., Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2016). No rapid audiovisual recalibration in adults on the autism spectrum. Nature Scientific Reports,6, 21756.CrossRef Turi, M., Karaminis, T., Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2016). No rapid audiovisual recalibration in adults on the autism spectrum. Nature Scientific Reports,6, 21756.CrossRef
go back to reference Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., & Belliveau, J. W. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,98, 11656–11661.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., & Belliveau, J. W. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,98, 11656–11661.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Van Boxtel, J. J., & Lu, H. (2013). Impaired global, and compensatory local, biological motion processing in people with high levels of autistic traits. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article ID 209. Van Boxtel, J. J., & Lu, H. (2013). Impaired global, and compensatory local, biological motion processing in people with high levels of autistic traits. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article ID 209.
go back to reference van Boxtel, J. J. A., Dapretto, M., & Lu, H. (2016). Intact recognition, but attenuated adaptation, for biological motion in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 9(10), 1103–1113.CrossRefPubMed van Boxtel, J. J. A., Dapretto, M., & Lu, H. (2016). Intact recognition, but attenuated adaptation, for biological motion in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 9(10), 1103–1113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., Van der Hallen, R., Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de Wit, L., et al. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological Review,121, 649–675.CrossRefPubMed van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., Van der Hallen, R., Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de Wit, L., et al. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological Review,121, 649–675.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wagenmakers, E. J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,14, 779–804.CrossRefPubMed Wagenmakers, E. J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,14, 779–804.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, L. H., Chien, S. H. L., Hu, S. F., Chen, T. Y., & Chen, H. S. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorders are less proficient in action identification and lacking a preference for upright point-light biological motion displays. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,11, 63–76.CrossRef Wang, L. H., Chien, S. H. L., Hu, S. F., Chen, T. Y., & Chen, H. S. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorders are less proficient in action identification and lacking a preference for upright point-light biological motion displays. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,11, 63–76.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, Y., Wang, L., Xu, Q., Liu, D., Chen, L., Troje, N. F., et al. (2018). Heritable aspects of biological motion perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA,115, 1937–1942.CrossRef Wang, Y., Wang, L., Xu, Q., Liu, D., Chen, L., Troje, N. F., et al. (2018). Heritable aspects of biological motion perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA,115, 1937–1942.CrossRef
go back to reference Watson, M. W. (1981). Maximum likelihood estimation of a moving average process via the EM algorithm. Mimeo. Watson, M. W. (1981). Maximum likelihood estimation of a moving average process via the EM algorithm. Mimeo.
go back to reference Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception and Psychophysics,33, 113–120.CrossRefPubMed Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception and Psychophysics,33, 113–120.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Webster, M. A., Werner, J. S., & Field, D. J. (2005). Adaptation and the phenomenology of vision. In C. Clifford & G. Rhodes (Eds.), Advances in cognition series. Fitting the mind to the world: Adaptation and aftereffects in high level vision (Vol. 2, pp. 241–277). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Webster, M. A., Werner, J. S., & Field, D. J. (2005). Adaptation and the phenomenology of vision. In C. Clifford & G. Rhodes (Eds.), Advances in cognition series. Fitting the mind to the world: Adaptation and aftereffects in high level vision (Vol. 2, pp. 241–277). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI -II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence-second edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI -II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence-second edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science,6, 291–298.CrossRefPubMed Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science,6, 291–298.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wilms, M., Schilbach, L., Pfeiffer, U., Bente, G., Fink, G. R., & Vogeley, K. (2010). It’s in your eyes—Using gaze-contingent stimuli to create truly interactive paradigms for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,5(1), 98–107.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wilms, M., Schilbach, L., Pfeiffer, U., Bente, G., Fink, G. R., & Vogeley, K. (2010). It’s in your eyes—Using gaze-contingent stimuli to create truly interactive paradigms for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,5(1), 98–107.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
Adaptation to the Speed of Biological Motion in Autism
Auteurs
Themis Karaminis
Roberto Arrighi
Georgia Forth
David Burr
Elizabeth Pellicano
Publicatiedatum
19-10-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 2/2020
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04241-4