Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2017

05-02-2016 | Original Article

Activation of context-specific attentional control sets by exogenous allocation of visual attention to the context?

Auteurs: Caroline Gottschalk, Rico Fischer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Different contexts with high versus low conflict frequencies require a specific attentional control involvement, i.e., strong attentional control for high conflict contexts and less attentional control for low conflict contexts. While it is assumed that the corresponding control set can be activated upon stimulus presentation at the respective context (e.g., upper versus lower location), the actual features that trigger control set activation are to date not described. Here, we ask whether the perceptual priming of the location context by an abrupt onset of irrelevant stimuli is sufficient in activating the context-specific attentional control set. For example, the mere onset of a stimulus might disambiguate the relevant location context and thus, serve as a low-level perceptual trigger mechanism that activates the context-specific attentional control set. In Experiment 1 and 2, the onsets of task-relevant and task-irrelevant (distracter) stimuli were manipulated at each context location to compete for triggering the activation of the appropriate control set. In Experiment 3, a prior training session enabled distracter stimuli to establish contextual control associations of their own before entering the test session. Results consistently showed that the mere onset of a task-irrelevant stimulus (with or without a context-control association) is not sufficient to activate the context-associated attentional control set by disambiguating the relevant context location. Instead, we argue that the identification of the relevant stimulus at the respective context is a precondition to trigger the activation of the context-associated attentional control set.
Voetnoten
1
Note, that for the study of RC compatibility effects the performance of Task 1 is the primary dependent measure. Performance in Task 2 is theoretically less important but is typically reported to control for trade-offs between tasks.
 
2
In a pilot study with equal font size, participants spontaneously reported that letters appeared larger in size than digits. This might be due to a wider horizontal extension of letters compared to digits. Therefore, we adjusted the size of letters to obtain a visually comparable impression.
 
3
It is conceivable, for example, that the requirement of re-orientation leads to a particular strong activation of the correct attentional control set, which might explain an increased CSPC effect in error rates of Block 2 in Experiment 3 when task-irrelevant stimuli appear first. We thank an anonymous reviewer for mentioning this possibility.
 
4
For the first block of Experiment 2, the CSPC effect (40 ms) was not significant, F(1, 19) = 1.68, p = .211 η p 2  = .08, which might be due to a power problem.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bugg, J. M. (2014a). Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 567.PubMed Bugg, J. M. (2014a). Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 567.PubMed
go back to reference Bugg, J. M. (2014b). Evidence for the sparing of reactive cognitive control with age. Psychology and Aging, 29(1), 115.CrossRefPubMed Bugg, J. M. (2014b). Evidence for the sparing of reactive cognitive control with age. Psychology and Aging, 29(1), 115.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bugg, J. M., & Hutchison, K. A. (2012). Converging evidence for control of color-word Stroop Interference at the item level. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,. doi:10.1037/a0029145.PubMed Bugg, J. M., & Hutchison, K. A. (2012). Converging evidence for control of color-word Stroop Interference at the item level. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,. doi:10.​1037/​a0029145.PubMed
go back to reference Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Chanani, S. (2011). Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 844. doi:10.1037/a0019957-2010-16967-001.PubMed Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Chanani, S. (2011). Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 844. doi:10.​1037/​a0019957-2010-16967-001.PubMed
go back to reference Cañadas, E., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Social categories as a context for the allocation of attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 934–943. doi:10.1037/a0029794-2012-22280-001.CrossRef Cañadas, E., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Social categories as a context for the allocation of attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 934–943. doi:10.​1037/​a0029794-2012-22280-001.CrossRef
go back to reference Corballis, P. M., & Gratton, G. (2003). Independent control of processing strategies for different locations in the visual field. Biological Psychology, 64(1–2), 191–209.CrossRefPubMed Corballis, P. M., & Gratton, G. (2003). Independent control of processing strategies for different locations in the visual field. Biological Psychology, 64(1–2), 191–209.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crump, M. J., Gong, Z., & Milliken, B. (2006). The context-specific proportion congruent Stroop effect: location as a contextual cue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(2), 316–321. doi:10.3758/Bf03193850.CrossRef Crump, M. J., Gong, Z., & Milliken, B. (2006). The context-specific proportion congruent Stroop effect: location as a contextual cue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(2), 316–321. doi:10.​3758/​Bf03193850.CrossRef
go back to reference Crump, M. J., & Milliken, B. (2009). The flexibility of context-specific control: evidence for context-driven generalization of item-specific control settings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1523–1532. doi:10.1080/17470210902752096.CrossRefPubMed Crump, M. J., & Milliken, B. (2009). The flexibility of context-specific control: evidence for context-driven generalization of item-specific control settings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1523–1532. doi:10.​1080/​1747021090275209​6.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, R., Miller, J., & Schubert, T. (2007). Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks. Memory & Cognition, 35(7), 1685–1699.CrossRef Fischer, R., Miller, J., & Schubert, T. (2007). Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks. Memory & Cognition, 35(7), 1685–1699.CrossRef
go back to reference Ghinescu, R., Schachtman, T. R., Stadler, M. A., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2010). Strategic behavior without awareness? Effects of implicit learning in the Eriksen flanker paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 38(2), 197–205. doi:10.3758/MC.38.2.19738/2/197.CrossRef Ghinescu, R., Schachtman, T. R., Stadler, M. A., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2010). Strategic behavior without awareness? Effects of implicit learning in the Eriksen flanker paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 38(2), 197–205. doi:10.​3758/​MC.​38.​2.​19738/​2/​197.CrossRef
go back to reference Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 638–644. doi:10.3758/Bf03196526.CrossRef Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 638–644. doi:10.​3758/​Bf03196526.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, M. S., & Cave, K. R. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: on the nature of interference from a salient distractor. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(6), 1009–1023.CrossRefPubMed Kim, M. S., & Cave, K. R. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: on the nature of interference from a salient distractor. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(6), 1009–1023.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 212–238. doi:10.3758/Bf03196277.CrossRef Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 212–238. doi:10.​3758/​Bf03196277.CrossRef
go back to reference Reder, L. M., Weber, K., Shang, J., & Vanyukov, P. M. (2003). The adaptive character of the attentional system: statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(3), 631–649.PubMedPubMedCentral Reder, L. M., Weber, K., Shang, J., & Vanyukov, P. M. (2003). The adaptive character of the attentional system: statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(3), 631–649.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Reuss, H., Desender, K., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Unconscious conflicts in unconscious contexts: the role of awareness and timing in flexible conflict adaptation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 1701–1718. doi:10.1037/a0036437-2014-12079-001.CrossRef Reuss, H., Desender, K., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Unconscious conflicts in unconscious contexts: the role of awareness and timing in flexible conflict adaptation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 1701–1718. doi:10.​1037/​a0036437-2014-12079-001.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 514. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514.CrossRefPubMed Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 514. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​34.​3.​514.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schreij, D., Owens, C., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 70(2), 208–218. doi:10.3758/Pp.70.2.208.CrossRef Schreij, D., Owens, C., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 70(2), 208–218. doi:10.​3758/​Pp.​70.​2.​208.CrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention (pp. 105–124). Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII. Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention (pp. 105–124). Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII.
go back to reference Walser, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2012). The failure of deactivating intentions: aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 1030–1044. doi:10.1037/a0027000-2012-02245-001.CrossRefPubMed Walser, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2012). The failure of deactivating intentions: aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 1030–1044. doi:10.​1037/​a0027000-2012-02245-001.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Vietze, I. (2008). Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 135–140.CrossRef Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Vietze, I. (2008). Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 135–140.CrossRef
go back to reference Wühr, P., & Kunde, W. (2008). Precueing spatial SR correspondence: is there regulation of expected response conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 872.PubMed Wühr, P., & Kunde, W. (2008). Precueing spatial SR correspondence: is there regulation of expected response conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 872.PubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S. (1998). Control of visual attention. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 223–256). Hove: Psychology. Yantis, S. (1998). Control of visual attention. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 223–256). Hove: Psychology.
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(1), 95. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.95.PubMed Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(1), 95. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​20.​1.​95.PubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601–621. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.601.PubMed Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601–621. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​10.​5.​601.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Activation of context-specific attentional control sets by exogenous allocation of visual attention to the context?
Auteurs
Caroline Gottschalk
Rico Fischer
Publicatiedatum
05-02-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0746-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2017

Psychological Research 2/2017 Naar de uitgave