Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2-3/2004

01-04-2004 | Original Article

Action induction through action observation

Auteurs: Sara De Maeght, Wolfgang Prinz

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2-3/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Ideomotor movements may arise in individuals while they watch goal-directed actions or events. In a previous study we developed a paradigm for investigating ideomotor movements induced through watching the outcome of one’s own action. In the present study we extended the paradigm to investigate both movements induced through watching the outcome of one’s own as well as somebody else’s action (player mode and observer mode respectively). We report three experiments, each with differing conditions for the player mode, but identical conditions for the observer mode. Results indicate that in both modes ideomotor movements are governed by two basic principles: Perceptual induction and intentional induction. In the player mode we replicated and extended previous findings, indicating dissociation between hand and head movements. In the observer mode no such dissociation was obtained. Our findings suggest that people perform, in their own actions, what they see being performed in other people’s actions. Induction of action through observation can pertain to both the action’s physical surface and underlying intentions. Furthermore, our results suggest that perceptual induction is ubiquitous but may be locally suspended for intentional action control. We discuss our results in the framework of theories invoking a strong overlap between representational structures for action perception and action planning.
Voetnoten
1
It may also be expected that ideomotor movements would be, in general, weaker and/or less frequent in monitoring the actions of others vs. self-performed actions (tracking vs. bowling). However, since the two tasks are quite different in their basic functional requirements, we believe that comparisons of absolute magnitudes of induced movements (i.e., main effects) do not make much sense. However, this argument does not affect comparisons of magnitudes of differences (i.e., interactions).
 
2
Randomized sequences of 192 event configurations from the bowling task were prepared as stimuli for each of the two conditions of the tracking task. These bowling patterns had actually been recorded from players’ performance in previous experiments. Depending on the task condition they showed initial corrective interventions either on the ball or the target. As in the bowling task, the sequences of 192 bowling patterns consisted of four replications of the 48 possible trajectories. Furthermore, these patterns exhibited an equal number of hits, left misses, and right misses (one-third each).
 
3
As is illustrated in Fig. 3 the beginning was set at the true onset of that period (0%), whereas the end was set at 80%. This latter point was chosen because pilot studies and earlier experiments had shown that in the very final phase of the induction period, induced movements may often be confounded with reset movements, i.e., movements that go back to the effector’s original default position (cf. Knuf et al., 2001, p. 787).
 
4
The application of the 200-ms standstill criterion could have one of two consequences, depending on whether or not a sufficiently long standstill was observed during the induction period of a trial. If there was no standstill of sufficient length, the trial was discarded. If there was a standstill of sufficient length, effective NIS values were computed from the net shift arising between the end of the standstill period and the 80% line. These effective NIS values were entered into the ANOVAs to be performed.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Bargh, J. A., & Barndollar, K. (1996). Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motives. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 457–481). New York: Guilford. Bargh, J. A., & Barndollar, K. (1996). Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motives. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 457–481). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology, with their applications to the training and discipline of the mind and the study of its morbid conditions. New York: Appleton. Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology, with their applications to the training and discipline of the mind and the study of its morbid conditions. New York: Appleton.
go back to reference Chevreul, M. E. (1833). Lettre à M. Ampère sur une classe particulière de mouvements musculaires. Revue des Deux Mondes, II, 258–266. Chevreul, M. E. (1833). Lettre à M. Ampère sur une classe particulière de mouvements musculaires. Revue des Deux Mondes, II, 258–266.
go back to reference Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (1998). The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Developmental Science, 1, 255–259.CrossRef Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (1998). The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Developmental Science, 1, 255–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biró, S., Koós, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.CrossRefPubMed Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biró, S., Koós, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., et al. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions: Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–1777.PubMed Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., et al. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions: Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–1777.PubMed
go back to reference Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–2611.PubMed Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–2611.PubMed
go back to reference Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.PubMed Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.PubMed
go back to reference Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance, Vol. XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 334–355). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance, Vol. XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 334–355). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research, 112, 103–111. Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research, 112, 103–111.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanism in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanism in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 52–57.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 52–57.PubMed
go back to reference Heider, F. (1926/1959). Thing and medium. Psychological Issues, 1959, Monograph 3 (pp. 1–34). [Original work published 1926.] Heider, F. (1926/1959). Thing and medium. Psychological Issues, 1959, Monograph 3 (pp. 1–34). [Original work published 1926.]
go back to reference Heider, F. (1930/1959). The function of the perceptual system. Psychological Issues, 1959, Monograph 3 (pp. 35–52). [Original work published 1930.] Heider, F. (1930/1959). The function of the perceptual system. Psychological Issues, 1959, Monograph 3 (pp. 35–52). [Original work published 1930.]
go back to reference Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). A theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. BBS—Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). A theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. BBS—Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Braß, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.PubMed Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Braß, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.PubMed
go back to reference James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vols. 1 & 2). New York: Holt. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vols. 1 & 2). New York: Holt.
go back to reference Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden actions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance, Vol. XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 356–380). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden actions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance, Vol. XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 356–380). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Jellema, T., Baker, C. I., Perrett, D. I., & Wicker, B. (2000). Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 205. Jellema, T., Baker, C. I., Perrett, D. I., & Wicker, B. (2000). Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 205.
go back to reference Jovanovic, B., Király, I., Elsner, B., Gergely, G., Prinz, W., & Aschersleben, G. (2003). The role of effects for infants’ perception of action goals. Manuscript in preparation. Jovanovic, B., Király, I., Elsner, B., Gergely, G., Prinz, W., & Aschersleben, G. (2003). The role of effects for infants’ perception of action goals. Manuscript in preparation.
go back to reference Katz, D. (1960). Sozialpsychologie. In D. Katz & R. Katz (Eds.), Handbuch der Psychologie. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe. Katz, D. (1960). Sozialpsychologie. In D. Katz & R. Katz (Eds.), Handbuch der Psychologie. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe.
go back to reference Knuf, L., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). An analysis of ideomotor action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 779–798.CrossRef Knuf, L., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). An analysis of ideomotor action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 779–798.CrossRef
go back to reference Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele [Medical psychology or mental physiology]. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung. Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele [Medical psychology or mental physiology]. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.
go back to reference Nishitani, N., & Hari, R. (2000). Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 97, 913–918.CrossRef Nishitani, N., & Hari, R. (2000). Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 97, 913–918.CrossRef
go back to reference Perrett, D. I., Harries, M. H., Bevan, R., Thomas, S., Benson, P. J., Mistlin, A. J., et al. (1989). Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. Journal of Experimental Biology, 146, 87–113. Perrett, D. I., Harries, M. H., Bevan, R., Thomas, S., Benson, P. J., Mistlin, A. J., et al. (1989). Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. Journal of Experimental Biology, 146, 87–113.
go back to reference Perrett, D. I., Harries, M. H., Mistlin, A. J., Hietänen, J. K., Benson, P. J., Bevan, R., et al. (1990). Social signals analysed at the single cell level: Someone is looking at me, something touched me, something moved. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 4, 25–50. Perrett, D. I., Harries, M. H., Mistlin, A. J., Hietänen, J. K., Benson, P. J., Bevan, R., et al. (1990). Social signals analysed at the single cell level: Someone is looking at me, something touched me, something moved. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 4, 25–50.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1987). Ideomotor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 47–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Prinz, W. (1987). Ideomotor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 47–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1990). A common-coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Prinz, W. (1990). A common-coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (2002). Experimental approaches to imitation. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 143–162). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Prinz, W. (2002). Experimental approaches to imitation. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 143–162). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Prinz, W., De Maeght, S., & Knuf, L. (in press). Intention in action. In G. Humphreys & J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in action: Advances from Cognitive Neuroscience. Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press. Prinz, W., De Maeght, S., & Knuf, L. (in press). Intention in action. In G. Humphreys & J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in action: Advances from Cognitive Neuroscience. Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research and Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.CrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research and Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670.CrossRefPubMed Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–740.CrossRef Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–740.CrossRef
go back to reference Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as in informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585–615. Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as in informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585–615.
go back to reference Scheerer, E. (1984). Motor theories of cognitive structure: A historical review. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 77–98). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Scheerer, E. (1984). Motor theories of cognitive structure: A historical review. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 77–98). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Stränger, J., & Hommel, B. (1995). Perception of action and movement. In W. Prinz & B. Bridgeman (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. I: Perception (pp. 397–451). London: Academic Press. Stränger, J., & Hommel, B. (1995). Perception of action and movement. In W. Prinz & B. Bridgeman (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, Vol. I: Perception (pp. 397–451). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.CrossRefPubMed Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145–160.CrossRef Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145–160.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Action induction through action observation
Auteurs
Sara De Maeght
Wolfgang Prinz
Publicatiedatum
01-04-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2-3/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0148-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2-3/2004

Psychological Research 2-3/2004 Naar de uitgave