Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2019

29-09-2018 | Original Article

Absence of age effects on spontaneous past and future thinking in daily life

Auteurs: Elizabeth Ann Warden, Benjamin Plimpton, Lia Kvavilashvili

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Previous research on voluntary mental time travel (i.e., deliberately thinking about the past or future) has resulted in negative age effects. In contrast, studies on spontaneous past thoughts (i.e., involuntary autobiographical memories) have reported small or no age effects. The aim of the present research was to investigate the effects of age on the nature and frequency of spontaneous future thoughts in everyday life. In two studies, we examined whether older adults reported spontaneous future thoughts as often as younger adults, and whether these thoughts were predominantly goal-oriented and less dependent on incidental cues than thoughts about the past. In Study 1, young and old participants kept a diary of spontaneous thoughts of upcoming prospective memory tasks and involuntary autobiographical memories for 2 weeks. In Study 2, a 1-day experience sampling method was used to investigate spontaneous and deliberate task-unrelated future and past thoughts, by having young and old participants complete a questionnaire in response to 30 random signals. In both studies, no age effects were found in the frequency of spontaneous future thoughts, which were reported as frequently as thoughts about the past (Study 2) and referred predominantly to upcoming tasks and goals rather than future events or hypothetical events (Study 2). Results concerning the triggers of reported thoughts provide initial support for the idea that representations of future thoughts may be more activated than memories of past events. Taken together, these results have important implications for research on cognitive ageing, mind-wandering, prospective memory and involuntary autobiographical memories.
Voetnoten
1
However, young and old groups did not differ in the percentage of participants who were employed (24% and 26%, respectively), and given that students often have a lot of self-managed time outside of university work, it could be argued that the groups were comparable and similar to the samples used in previous naturalistic and experience sampling studies of spontaneous past and future thoughts (e.g., Berntsen & Jacobsen, 2008; Gardner & Ascoli, 2015; Kvavilashvili & Fisher, 2007).
 
2
A small percentage of entries were reported at times that occurred before the prompt time, and may have been due to simple perceptual errors or time differences in participants’ own clocks. These cases were excluded from the percentage of compliance times recorded above.
 
3
The finding that 23% of the thoughts sampled were task-unrelated is modest compared to other experience sampling studies that indicated that people spent between 25% and 50% of their day engaged in task-unrelated thought (Kane et al., 2007; Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). Previous studies, however, relied on participants’ own categorization of their thoughts, which could have resulted in biases (e.g., incorrectly interpreting task-related interference as mind-wandering). Additionally, the current study adopted very stringent criteria, and borderline cases were coded as task-related rather than task-unrelated.
 
4
For laboratory studies in which the Intention Superiority Effect is measured via speeded reaction times to the content of to-be-carried out tasks see Dockree and Ellis (2001), Goschke and Kuhl (1993) and Marsh, Hicks and Bink (1998).
 
5
It is, however, worth pointing out that we chose one non-working day to make the samples of young and old participants more comparable in terms of the nature of activities involved.
 
6
Our sample sizes were comparable to those used in previous diary studies of involuntary past and future thinking in which 20–25 participants have been used per condition (e.g., Berntsen & Jacobsen, 2008; Finnbogadóttir, & Berntsen, 2011; Kvavilashvili & Fisher, 2007; Rasmussen & Berntsen, 2011). Power calculations using G*Power 3 (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang & Buchner, 2007), showed that with the sample size we had in Study 1 (based on at least 19 participants in each condition), for the 2 age (young vs. old) x 2 thought type (past vs. future) mixed ANOVA, the power to detect a medium to large effect of age as measured by partial eta-squared (ηp2 = 0.10) was 0.61, and the power to detect an interaction of medium size (ηp2 = 0.06) was 0.85 (with the correlation between the dependent variables of 0.58 and the non sphericity correction of 1.00). In Study 2, with the existing sample size of at least 22 participants per group, in the 2 age (young vs. old) a 5 thought type (Task-related, task-unrelated interference, task-unrelated, external distraction, no thought) mixed ANOVA, we had the power of 0.80 to detect a medium size age effect of ηp2 = 0.06, and the power of 0.75 to detect a medium to large size interaction of ηp2 = 0.10 (with the correlation between the dependent measurements of 0.17 and the sphericity correction of 0.25). These calculations show that we had sufficient power to detect medium to large size affects that have been often reported in the cognitive ageing literature.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aberle, I., Rendell, P. G., Rose, N. S., McDaniel, M. A., & Kliegel, M. (2010). The age prospective memory paradox: Young adults may not give their best outside of the lab. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1444–1453.CrossRef Aberle, I., Rendell, P. G., Rose, N. S., McDaniel, M. A., & Kliegel, M. (2010). The age prospective memory paradox: Young adults may not give their best outside of the lab. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1444–1453.CrossRef
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F., Hofmann, W., & Vohs, K. D. (2015). Everyday thoughts about the past, present, and future: An experience sampling study of mental time travel (manuscript under review). Baumeister, R. F., Hofmann, W., & Vohs, K. D. (2015). Everyday thoughts about the past, present, and future: An experience sampling study of mental time travel (manuscript under review).
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Pragmatic prospection: How and why people think about the future. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 3–16.CrossRef Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Pragmatic prospection: How and why people think about the future. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 3–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Craik, F. I. M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in memory. In: F. Klix and H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performances (pp. 409–422). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Craik, F. I. M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in memory. In: F. Klix and H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performances (pp. 409–422). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Dismukes, K. (2008). Prospective memory in everyday and aviation settings. In M. Kliegel, M. A. M. A. McDaniel & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 411–431). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Dismukes, K. (2008). Prospective memory in everyday and aviation settings. In M. Kliegel, M. A. M. A. McDaniel & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 411–431). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39.CrossRef Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39.CrossRef
go back to reference Ihle, A., Schnitzspahn, K., Rendell, P. G., Luong, C., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Age benefits in everyday prospective memory: The influence of personal task importance, use of reminders and everyday stress. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19, 84–101.CrossRef Ihle, A., Schnitzspahn, K., Rendell, P. G., Luong, C., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Age benefits in everyday prospective memory: The influence of personal task importance, use of reminders and everyday stress. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19, 84–101.CrossRef
go back to reference Kvavilashvili, L., Cockburn, J., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2013). Prospective memory and ageing paradox with event-based tasks: A study of young, young-old, and old-old participants. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 864–875.CrossRef Kvavilashvili, L., Cockburn, J., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2013). Prospective memory and ageing paradox with event-based tasks: A study of young, young-old, and old-old participants. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 864–875.CrossRef
go back to reference Kvavilashvili, L., Niedźwieńska, A., & Kliegel, M. (2016). Do older adults have fewer involuntary autobiographical memories than younger adults? Contrasting evidence from different laboratory methods. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Boston, November. Kvavilashvili, L., Niedźwieńska, A., & Kliegel, M. (2016). Do older adults have fewer involuntary autobiographical memories than younger adults? Contrasting evidence from different laboratory methods. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Boston, November.
go back to reference Laughland, A., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2018). Should participants be left to their own devices? Comparing paper and smartphone diaries in psychological research. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (in press). Laughland, A., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2018). Should participants be left to their own devices? Comparing paper and smartphone diaries in psychological research. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Maylor, E. A., Darby, R. J., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Retrieval of performed versus to-be-performed tasks: A naturalistic study of the intention-superiority effect in normal aging and dementia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S83–S98.CrossRef Maylor, E. A., Darby, R. J., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Retrieval of performed versus to-be-performed tasks: A naturalistic study of the intention-superiority effect in normal aging and dementia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S83–S98.CrossRef
go back to reference Mullet, H. G., Scullin, M. K., Hess, T. J., Scullin, R. B., Arnold, K. M., & Einstein, G. O. (2013). Prospective memory and aging: Evidence for preserved spontaneous retrieval with exact but not related cues. Psychology & Aging, 28, 910–922. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034347.CrossRef Mullet, H. G., Scullin, M. K., Hess, T. J., Scullin, R. B., Arnold, K. M., & Einstein, G. O. (2013). Prospective memory and aging: Evidence for preserved spontaneous retrieval with exact but not related cues. Psychology & Aging, 28, 910–922. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0034347.CrossRef
go back to reference Niedźwieńska, A., & Barzykowski, K. (2012). The age prospective memory paradox within the same sample in time-based and event-based tasks. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 58–83.CrossRef Niedźwieńska, A., & Barzykowski, K. (2012). The age prospective memory paradox within the same sample in time-based and event-based tasks. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 58–83.CrossRef
go back to reference Niedźwieńska, A., Janik, B., & Jarczyńska, A. (2013). Age-related differences in everyday prospective memory tasks: The role of planning and personal importance. International Journal of Psychology, 48, 1291–1302.CrossRef Niedźwieńska, A., Janik, B., & Jarczyńska, A. (2013). Age-related differences in everyday prospective memory tasks: The role of planning and personal importance. International Journal of Psychology, 48, 1291–1302.CrossRef
go back to reference Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., & Martin, M. (2008). Adult aging and prospective memory: The importance of ecological validity. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 161–185). London: Lawrence Erlbaum. Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., & Martin, M. (2008). Adult aging and prospective memory: The importance of ecological validity. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 161–185). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Prince, M. J., Macdonald, A. M., Sham, P. C., Richards, M., Quraishi, S., & Horn, I. (1999). The development and initial validation of a telephone-administered cognitive test battery (TACT). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 8(1), 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.56.CrossRef Prince, M. J., Macdonald, A. M., Sham, P. C., Richards, M., Quraishi, S., & Horn, I. (1999). The development and initial validation of a telephone-administered cognitive test battery (TACT). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 8(1), 49–57. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​mpr.​56.CrossRef
go back to reference Schnitzspahn, K. M., Ihle, A., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., & Kliegel, M. (2011). The age-prospective memory-paradox: A comprehensive exploration of possible mechanisms. International Psychogeriatrics, 23, 583–592.CrossRef Schnitzspahn, K. M., Ihle, A., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., & Kliegel, M. (2011). The age-prospective memory-paradox: A comprehensive exploration of possible mechanisms. International Psychogeriatrics, 23, 583–592.CrossRef
go back to reference Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Xu, M., Purdon, C., & Smilek, D. (2015). Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 41(5), 1417–1425. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000116.CrossRef Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Xu, M., Purdon, C., & Smilek, D. (2015). Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 41(5), 1417–1425. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​xlm0000116.CrossRef
go back to reference Stawarczyk, D. (2018). The phenomenology of mind-wandering and daydreaming: A historical overview and functional correlates. In K. C. R. Fox & K. Christoff (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought. New York: Oxford University Press. Stawarczyk, D. (2018). The phenomenology of mind-wandering and daydreaming: A historical overview and functional correlates. In K. C. R. Fox & K. Christoff (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Weinstein, Y. (2018). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 642–661.CrossRef Weinstein, Y. (2018). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 642–661.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Absence of age effects on spontaneous past and future thinking in daily life
Auteurs
Elizabeth Ann Warden
Benjamin Plimpton
Lia Kvavilashvili
Publicatiedatum
29-09-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1103-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2019

Psychological Research 4/2019 Naar de uitgave