Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2-3/2004

01-04-2004 | Original Article

A short history of ideo-motor action

Auteurs: Armin Stock, Claudia Stock

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2-3/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The ideo-motor theory, which is currently receiving heightened interest in cognitive psychology, looks back on a long history. Essentially two historical roots can be presented. A British one, initiated by Laycock (1845) and Carpenter (1852), which was developed in order to explain ideo-motor phenomena by means of cerebral reflex actions. A second and older root is the German one by Herbart (1816, 1825), Lotze (1852), and Harless (1861), which considered the ideo-motor principle a fundamental mechanism of all intentional human behaviour. Both roots converged in James’ (1890) Principles of Psychology before they fell into oblivion due to the dominance of behaviorism in the first half of the 20th century. The few empirical ideo-motor studies of the early 20th century are briefly described. Finally, similarities and differences in the history of the ideo-motor theory are delineated and a perspective is given covering research questions that could be examined in the future.
Voetnoten
1
According to Wozniak (1999) other members of this research group were: Benjamin Collins Brodie (1783–1862), Robert Dunn (1799–1877), Henry Holland (1788–1873), Thomas Laycock (1812–1876) John Daniel Morell (1816–1891), and Daniel Noble (1810–1885).
 
2
In the history of psychology the conception of physiological psychology is usually ascribed to Wilhelm Wundt (1873/1874). This is not entirely correct when considering the work of the research-group around Carpenter.
 
3
Nevertheless, Carpenter denied that Laycock had already described the triggering of cerebral reflexes by ideas and presented these findings of Laycock as his own. This resulted in an ongoing dispute concerning authorship between the two (cf. Carpenter, 1871; Laycock, 1860, 1876).
 
4
But one could suppose that Lotze was confronted with the works of the anatomist Albrecht von Haller (1708–1777) during his medical studies. Professor von Haller held a position in the Department of Anatomy, Surgery, and Botany at the University of Göttingen from 1736 till 1753 (cf. Beug, 1977). As mentioned earlier, Laycock (1845) had also made reference to von Haller as a scholar who supported the substrate theory. Maybe von Haller is the connecting link between Laycock and Lotze.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allers, R., & Scheminzky, F. (1926). Über Aktionsströme der Muskeln bei motorischen Vorstellungen und verwandten Vorgängen. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 212, 169–182. Allers, R., & Scheminzky, F. (1926). Über Aktionsströme der Muskeln bei motorischen Vorstellungen und verwandten Vorgängen. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 212, 169–182.
go back to reference Beug, H.-J. (1977). Albrecht von Haller an der Georg-August-Universität Göttingen. In Albrecht von Haller. Zum 200. Todestag. Göttingen, Germany: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht. Beug, H.-J. (1977). Albrecht von Haller an der Georg-August-Universität Göttingen. In Albrecht von Haller. Zum 200. Todestag. Göttingen, Germany: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.
go back to reference Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394–409. Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394–409.
go back to reference Carpenter, W. B. (1852). On the influence of suggestion in modifying and directing muscular movement, independently of volition. Proceedings of the Royal Institution, 147–154. Carpenter, W. B. (1852). On the influence of suggestion in modifying and directing muscular movement, independently of volition. Proceedings of the Royal Institution, 147–154.
go back to reference Carpenter, W. B. (1871). Spiritualism and its recent converts. Quarterly Review, 131, 301–353. Carpenter, W. B. (1871). Spiritualism and its recent converts. Quarterly Review, 131, 301–353.
go back to reference Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. London: Herny S. King. Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. London: Herny S. King.
go back to reference Carus, C. G. (1988). Über Lebensmagnetismus und über die magischen Wirkungen überhaupt. Andechs, Germany: Dingfelder. Carus, C. G. (1988). Über Lebensmagnetismus und über die magischen Wirkungen überhaupt. Andechs, Germany: Dingfelder.
go back to reference Chevreul, M.-E. (1833). Lettre à M. Ampère sur une classe particulière de mouvements musculaires. Revue des Deux Mondes, 2, 249–257. Chevreul, M.-E. (1833). Lettre à M. Ampère sur une classe particulière de mouvements musculaires. Revue des Deux Mondes, 2, 249–257.
go back to reference Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1990). Effect of reinforcer devaluation on discriminative control of instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 40–47.PubMed Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1990). Effect of reinforcer devaluation on discriminative control of instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 40–47.PubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, 27, 229–240. Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, 27, 229–240.
go back to reference Faraday, M. (1853). Experimental investigation of table-moving. Athenaeum, 1340, 801–803. Faraday, M. (1853). Experimental investigation of table-moving. Athenaeum, 1340, 801–803.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed
go back to reference Harless, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. In I. H. Fichte, H. Ulrici, & I. U. Wirth (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik: Vol. 38 (pp. 50–73). Halle, Germany: Pfeffer. Harless, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. In I. H. Fichte, H. Ulrici, & I. U. Wirth (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik: Vol. 38 (pp. 50–73). Halle, Germany: Pfeffer.
go back to reference Herbart, J. F. (1816). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Königsberg, Germany: Unzer. Herbart, J. F. (1816). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Königsberg, Germany: Unzer.
go back to reference Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik. Zweiter, analytischer Teil. Königsberg, Germany: Unzer. Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik. Zweiter, analytischer Teil. Königsberg, Germany: Unzer.
go back to reference Herbart, J. F. (1834/1965). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Nachdruck der Ausgabe Königsberg 1834. Amsterdam: Bonset. Herbart, J. F. (1834/1965). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Nachdruck der Ausgabe Königsberg 1834. Amsterdam: Bonset.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2000a). Antizipative Verhaltenskontrolle. In B. Strauss & J. Möller (Eds.), Psychologie und Zukunft (pp. 1–29). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2000a). Antizipative Verhaltenskontrolle. In B. Strauss & J. Möller (Eds.), Psychologie und Zukunft (pp. 1–29). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2000b). Lernmechanismen zum Erwerb verhaltenssteuernden Wissens. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 1–9.CrossRef Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2000b). Lernmechanismen zum Erwerb verhaltenssteuernden Wissens. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., & Stock, A. (2000). Intention als psychischer Prozess—eine Suche nach Spuren in der allgemeinpsychologischen Forschung. In K. Friedhart (Ed.), Psychologie 2000 (pp. 1–24). Berlin, Germany: J. A. Barth. Hoffmann, J., & Stock, A. (2000). Intention als psychischer Prozess—eine Suche nach Spuren in der allgemeinpsychologischen Forschung. In K. Friedhart (Ed.), Psychologie 2000 (pp. 1–24). Berlin, Germany: J. A. Barth.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482. Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.PubMed Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Perceiving one’s own action—and what it leads to. In J. S. Jordan (Ed.), Systems theory and a priori aspects of perception (pp. 143–179). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Hommel, B. (1998). Perceiving one’s own action—and what it leads to. In J. S. Jordan (Ed.), Systems theory and a priori aspects of perception (pp. 143–179). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2003). Acquisition and control of voluntary action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hommel, B. (2003). Acquisition and control of voluntary action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed
go back to reference Hyman, R. (1999). The mischief making of ideomotor action. Scientific Review of Alternative Medicine, 3, 30–39. Hyman, R. (1999). The mischief making of ideomotor action. Scientific Review of Alternative Medicine, 3, 30–39.
go back to reference James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, Vols. I, II. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, Vols. I, II. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Külpe, O. (1889). Die Lehre vom Willen in der neueren Psychologie. In W. Wundt (Ed.), Philosophische Studien, Vol. 5. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann. Külpe, O. (1889). Die Lehre vom Willen in der neueren Psychologie. In W. Wundt (Ed.), Philosophische Studien, Vol. 5. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394. Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.
go back to reference Laycock, T. (1840). A treatise on the nervous diseases of women; comprising an inquiry into the nature, causes, and treatment of spinal and hysterical disorders. London: Longmans. Laycock, T. (1840). A treatise on the nervous diseases of women; comprising an inquiry into the nature, causes, and treatment of spinal and hysterical disorders. London: Longmans.
go back to reference Laycock, T. (1845). On the reflex functions of the brain. In Reprinted from N. XXXVII of The British and Foreign Medical Review (pp. 1–16). Bartholomew Close, UK: Adlard. Laycock, T. (1845). On the reflex functions of the brain. In Reprinted from N. XXXVII of The British and Foreign Medical Review (pp. 1–16). Bartholomew Close, UK: Adlard.
go back to reference Laycock, T. (1860). Mind and brain: or the correlations of consciousness and organisation; with their applications to philosophy, zoology, physiology, mental pathology, and the practice of medicine, Vols. I, II. Edinburgh/London: Sutherland and Knox/Simpkin, Marshall, and Co. Laycock, T. (1860). Mind and brain: or the correlations of consciousness and organisation; with their applications to philosophy, zoology, physiology, mental pathology, and the practice of medicine, Vols. I, II. Edinburgh/London: Sutherland and Knox/Simpkin, Marshall, and Co.
go back to reference Laycock, T. (1876). Reflex, automatic, and unconscious cerebration: A history and a criticism. The Journal of Mental Science, 21, 477–498. Laycock, T. (1876). Reflex, automatic, and unconscious cerebration: A history and a criticism. The Journal of Mental Science, 21, 477–498.
go back to reference Lotze, R. H. (1841). Metaphysik. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann. Lotze, R. H. (1841). Metaphysik. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann.
go back to reference Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele (pp. 287–325). Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung. Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele (pp. 287–325). Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.
go back to reference Moede, W. (1920). Experimentelle Massenpsychologie. Beiträge zur Experimentalpsychologie der Gruppe. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel. Moede, W. (1920). Experimentelle Massenpsychologie. Beiträge zur Experimentalpsychologie der Gruppe. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.
go back to reference Nath, M. (1892). Die Psychologie Hermann Lotzes in ihrem Verhältnis zu Herbart. Inaugural Dissertation. Halle a.S., Germany. Nath, M. (1892). Die Psychologie Hermann Lotzes in ihrem Verhältnis zu Herbart. Inaugural Dissertation. Halle a.S., Germany.
go back to reference Pesters, R. (1997). Hermann Lotze. Wege seines Denkens und Forschens. Würzburg, Germany: Königshausen & Neumann. Pesters, R. (1997). Hermann Lotze. Wege seines Denkens und Forschens. Würzburg, Germany: Königshausen & Neumann.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1987). Ideo-motor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 47–76). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Prinz, W. (1987). Ideo-motor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 47–76). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
go back to reference Rescorla, R. A. (1990a). Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 326–334. Rescorla, R. A. (1990a). Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 326–334.
go back to reference Rescorla, R. A. (1990b). The role of information about the response-outcome relation in instrumental discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 262–270. Rescorla, R. A. (1990b). The role of information about the response-outcome relation in instrumental discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 262–270.
go back to reference Rescorla, R. A. (1991a). Associations of multiple outcomes with instrumental response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Processes, 17, 465–474. Rescorla, R. A. (1991a). Associations of multiple outcomes with instrumental response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Processes, 17, 465–474.
go back to reference Rescorla, R. A. (1991b). Associative relations in instrumental learning: The Eighteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43b, 1–23. Rescorla, R. A. (1991b). Associative relations in instrumental learning: The Eighteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43b, 1–23.
go back to reference Richter, H. (1954). Über ideomotorische Phänomene. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 157, 201–257. Richter, H. (1954). Über ideomotorische Phänomene. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 157, 201–257.
go back to reference Richter, H. (1957). Zum Problem der ideomotorischen Phänomene. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 161, 161–254. Richter, H. (1957). Zum Problem der ideomotorischen Phänomene. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 161, 161–254.
go back to reference Sommer, R. (1898). Dreidimensionale Analyse von Ausdrucksbewegungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 16, 275. Sommer, R. (1898). Dreidimensionale Analyse von Ausdrucksbewegungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 16, 275.
go back to reference Spitz, H. H. (1997). Nonconscious movements. From mystical messages to facilitated communication. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Spitz, H. H. (1997). Nonconscious movements. From mystical messages to facilitated communication. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Stock, A. (2002). Intentionalität und Ideo-Motorik. Eine handlungstheoretisch-psychologische Synthese. Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, Habilitationsschrift. Stock, A. (2002). Intentionalität und Ideo-Motorik. Eine handlungstheoretisch-psychologische Synthese. Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, Habilitationsschrift.
go back to reference Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R-O learning blocks S-R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.CrossRef Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R-O learning blocks S-R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.CrossRef
go back to reference Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Ideo-motor action. Psychological Review, 20, 91–106. Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Ideo-motor action. Psychological Review, 20, 91–106.
go back to reference Tischner, R. (1929). Zur Geschichte des ideomotorischen Prinzips. Zeitschrift für Parapsychologie, 56, 75–85; 155–161. Tischner, R. (1929). Zur Geschichte des ideomotorischen Prinzips. Zeitschrift für Parapsychologie, 56, 75–85; 155–161.
go back to reference Wentscher, M. (1925). Fechner und Lotze. Munich, Germany: Ernst Reinhardt. Wentscher, M. (1925). Fechner und Lotze. Munich, Germany: Ernst Reinhardt.
go back to reference Wozniak, R. H. (1999). Classics in psychology. 1855–1914: Historical essays. Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press. Wozniak, R. H. (1999). Classics in psychology. 1855–1914: Historical essays. Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press.
go back to reference Wundt, W. (1873/1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Wilhelm Engelmann. Wundt, W. (1873/1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Wilhelm Engelmann.
go back to reference Young, R. M. (1990). The mind-body problem. In: R. C. Olby et al. (Eds.), Companion to the history of modern science. London: Routledge. Young, R. M. (1990). The mind-body problem. In: R. C. Olby et al. (Eds.), Companion to the history of modern science. London: Routledge.
go back to reference Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978. Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.
go back to reference Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 595–613. Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 595–613.
Metagegevens
Titel
A short history of ideo-motor action
Auteurs
Armin Stock
Claudia Stock
Publicatiedatum
01-04-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2-3/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0154-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2-3/2004

Psychological Research 2-3/2004 Naar de uitgave