Introduction
In addition to the most striking lifelong effects of impaired communication, socialization and restrictive/repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), there is a high prevalence of sensory perceptual anomalies (Baranek
2002). Evidence for which has relied on autobiographical, observational or behavioural measures (Moore
2015) which has demonstrated, amongst an array of sensory disturbances, an absence of typical pain behaviours (e.g. absence of hand withdrawal reflex or a lack of protective body positioning) when encountering pain (Bursch et al.
2004; Gillberg and Coleman
2000; Mahler
1952; Rothenberg
1960; Wing
1996). There is further evidence that autistic individuals have aversions to touch (Grandin
1992,
1995; Williams
2015), suggesting that light tactile sensation might be a source of discomfort, indicating a potential hypersensitivity to tactile stimuli (Kaiser et al.
2016; Moore
2015). However, such methods are typically not generalizable because it is unclear whether the case investigated is representative of the wider body of “similar” instances. Further validation of this phenomenon is given by the re-incorporation of sensory responses as a feature in diagnostic texts suggesting that it is a central clinical finding in autism (APA
2013). There is however, a dearth of rigorous psychophysical experimental evidence to support these claims. Therefore, the current study aims to clarify the characteristics of
pain sensitivity associated with ASD using a psychophysically robust experimental case-control design.
Pain is multifaceted, defined as a distressing experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage; with sensory, emotional, cognitive and social components (IASP
2012; Williams and Craig
2016). Together, the percept, and the subjective reaction act as a warning system so that individuals learn to avoid dangerous stimuli (Yasuda et al.
2016), whilst also promoting behavioural analgesia (Eccleston and Crombez
1999). A disruption to this system could result in a lack of these learned behaviours.
Potentially nociceptive (painful) stimuli are detected by specific somatosensory receptor neurons (nerve fibres), known as nociceptors which can be classified into three different types: Aβ, Aδ and C-fibre (Besson
1999; Delmas et al.
2011; Djouhri and Lawson
2004; Lumpkin and Caterina
2007). Nociceptive messages are typically mediated by Aδ, and C-fibres, the functionality of which, in neurotypical populations, has been well described (for reviews see Basbaum et al.
2009; Basbaum and Jessell
2000; McGlone and Reilly
2010; Meyer et al.
2008). Before these signals generate a perception of ‘pain’ they are centrally integrated in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord and transmitted to the brain via the spinothalamic tract (Basbaum and Jessell
2000; Iggo
1977; Nafe
1934; Schiller
1956). The final process in the pain experience is the social communication of pain which can be observed in stereotyped pain behaviours (Craig
2015) and self-report – and which is neither simply, nor directly, associated with the level of nociceptor activity; nociceptor activity can produce more or less pain depending on a range of factors (Loeser
2012). De-coding whether these underlying mechanisms are altered in autistic individuals will give insight into the pain behaviours observed in this population.
Recently a few studies have begun to disentangle the underlying sensory mechanisms of somatosensory dysfunctions in ASD using psychophysical methods, the earliest of which focused on tactile sensitivity, investigating this with vibrotactile stimuli (Blakemore et al.
2006; Cascio et al.
2008; Guclu et al.
2007). Blakemore et al. (
2006) reported a frequency dependent hypersensitivity in adults with Asperger’s compared to neurotypical controls. Conversely, Guclu et al. (
2007) and Cascio et al. (
2008) report no significant difference between the vibrotactile thresholds of children and adults with ASD and controls, suggesting that effects may be a result of specific frequencies, sites or other methodological differences.
Regarding pain perception, the focus has generally been towards thermal testing, with similarly mixed findings. Thermal pain hypersensitivity but normal thermal detection has been reported in adults with ASD (Cascio et al.
2008). Adolescents are reported to have the inverse results; normal thermal pain thresholds, but a hyposensitivity to innocuous thermal stimuli (Duerden et al.
2015). No differences in thermal detection thresholds and electrical pain were observed by Yasuda et al. (
2016) and Bird et al. (
2010), however, pressure pain thresholds were lower in autistic individuals compared to controls (Fan et al.
2014). This pattern of findings suggests no systematic change in psychophysically determined pain thresholds for autistic individuals compared to controls. This is not to suggest that pain response in ASD is typical, both Fründt et al. (
2017) and Duerden et al. (
2015) report paradoxical heat sensations, a phenomenon where gentle cooling is perceived as hot or burning (Magerl and Klein
2006), in several of their autistic participants. This phenomenon usually does not occur in healthy individuals.
Considering the paucity of evidence paired with the mixed results, probably due to the heterogeneity of participants (e.g. autism symptom severity or comorbidities) and differences regarding methods and sub-modalities investigated, the disentanglement of the underlying mechanisms of somatosensory dysfunctions in ASD is limited and there is no gold standard on how these features should be assessed in ASD.
Several recent investigations (Blakemore et al.
2006; Cascio et al.
2008; Duerden et al.
2015) have utilised methodologies that have been collated into the standardised quantitative sensory testing battery developed by The German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS; Rolke et al.
2006). This method allows for the quantification of clinically significant perception and pain thresholds (Werner et al.
2013) assessing the function of small and large diameter nerve fibres (Hansson et al.
2007). If used in its entirety this method allows researchers to assess nerve function across the full range of modalities; vibration, pressure, thermal, and mechanical (Moloney et al.
2012) in a standardised manner. The focus on a single or a limited number of these sub-modalities limits previous studies. One study, however, has utilised this full battery, therefore, providing the most comprehensive assessment of somatosensory function in ASD to date (Fründt et al.
2017). More extreme somatosensory responses (i.e. hyper- or hypo-sensitivity) or somatosensory phenomena not typically observed in those without neuropathy (i.e. dynamic mechanical allodynia or paradoxical heat sensations) were observed in the ASD group, however, there were no group differences reported for global or systemic changes in somatosensory function.
This study will similarly employ the standardised battery, conducting an independent replication of Fründt et al. (
2017) and utilise the published normative reference values (Rolke et al.
2006) as they provide a determinant of sensory loss and gain that supersedes the standard group differences analysis - meaning clinically significant sensitivities in ASD can be determined. Furthermore, this battery has been extended to include a measure of pain tolerance and central pain processes, utilising the cold pressor test (von Baeyer et al.
2005) and Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM; Yarnitsky et al.
2015), respectively. Including tolerance allows a wider range of psychophysics to be measured; threshold (the minimum intensity of a stimulus that is perceived as painful), suprathreshold (increases the frequency of nociceptive messages) to tolerance (the maximum intensity of a pain-producing stimulus that a subject is willing to accept in a given situation (Chapman et al.
1985; IASP
2012). Tolerance also includes additional components such as pain motivation; to quantify said motivation; self-reported desires to avoid pain were measured. CPM represents one type of central pain process; that of descending spinal modulation, that although not currently tested in ASD populations, is a paradigm easily implemented in a laboratory setting. It is a process whereby one noxious stimulus inhibits the perception of a second noxious stimulus, where greater reductions in pain are thought to reflect greater pain inhibitory capacity (Martel et al.
2013; Nir and Yarnitsky
2015). The addition of each will give insight into tolerance, pain motivation, and central pain processes in ASD.
Discussion
The current study investigated somatosensory perception in autistic individuals to test the hypothesis that the different pain behaviours observed in anecdotal accounts were the result of an alteration in somatosensory mechanisms. For this reason, and to allow the comparison to published norms, 13 autistic adults and 13 age- and gender- matched control participants without autism, underwent a standardised and normed QST protocol (DFNS: Rolke et al.
2006). No observable consistent pathological QST pattern suggesting a defined nerve fibre dysfunction, which could account for the altered pain behaviours observed, was found. The ASD group showed no systematic changes in their QST pattern.
Group differences were found, however, for both mechanical pain threshold (MPT; pinprick stimuli) and mechanical detection threshold (MDT; von Frey filaments), with the ASD group showing higher thresholds for both. Although the ASD group had higher thresholds compared to the control group, data for both groups reside within the normal distribution of healthy individuals, as established by the DFNS, indicating that although the ASD group may be less sensitive to mechanical pain than controls this sensitivity is not clinically significant. However, ASD group mean value for MDT fell outside the normative range for healthy individuals, suggesting a clinically significant degree of sensory loss at the group level. Normal z scores for other clinically related QST parameters—such as vibration detection threshold—do suggest, however, typical Aβ-fibre function (Grone et al.
2012).
Vibrotactile and punctate stimulation are both communicated via Aβ-fibres, though detected by different receptor pathways, which may account for the aforementioned differences. High frequency vibration is detected via rapidly adapting Pacinian corpuscle and generally have a large receptive field. Mechanical stimulation, on the other hand, are detected via slowly adapting Merkel cell-neurite complex receptors and is tactile detection via indentation depth (Delmas et al.
2011). Different Aβ-fibre phenotypic alterations may therefore be present and be stimuli specific, due to detection of such stimuli by their specific receptors. Such differences are highlighted in the evidence when contrary to the sensory loss of MDT measured by von Frey, increased sensitivity to vibration is reported (Cascio et al.
2008). There is greater difficulty in comparing vibration results in the literature, due to the varied vibration frequencies used (Blakemore et al.
2006; Guclu et al.
2007), yielding very different results which may similarly be a result of different receptor activation (Lumpkin et al.
2010; McGlone and Reilly
2010; McGlone et al.
2014). It must also be noted that the use of a tuning fork for vibrotactile assessment is sensitive enough to identify neuropathy—as intended—however, may not be sensitive enough to measure more subtle changes in threshold. Findings for MDT are in line with Fründt et al. (
2017) who similarly report a significant loss of function for mechanical detection in ASD participants using the same standardised testing from the QST battery.
Similar to Fründt et al. (
2017) who report PHS and DMA in two autistic individuals (see also Duerden et al.
2015), three participants showed distinctive sensory features in the form of paradoxical heat sensations (n = 1; PHS) and dynamic mechanical allodynia (n = 2; DMA), that do not usually occur in healthy individuals on the upper limbs (Rolke et al.
2006) and were not observed in the control group. Given that the different QST parameters did not reveal any specific signs of nerve fibre dysfunction in both studies, we concur with the author’s suggestion that central mechanisms determine PHS in the ASD groups. Studies of patients with CNS demyelination confirm central processing issues that result in PHS (Hansen et al.
1996). Limited research has attempted to understand the central processing of pain in ASD using neuroimaging techniques. This research supports the idea that changes in pain processing in ASD is complex: suggesting that there is an initial processing which is similar to controls, however, there is a reduction in neural activity during sustained pain that is not present in controls (Failla et al.
2017). This gives further support to the need to be flexible about how pain experience is considered in ASD.
A further phenomenon, observed by this study and that of Fründt et al. (
2017) is that of DMA. Both studies are the first to experimentally measure DMA in ASD, observing this in a subset of the ASD groups. DMA is the experience of perceiving innocuous touch, such as gentle stroking, as aversive, a phenomenon observed in ASD sensory over-responsivity literature (Baranek and Berkson
1994; Green et al.
2015; Reynolds and Lane
2008). Central sensitisation i.e. changes in signalling in the spinal cord (Campbell and Meyer
2006), is commonly thought to underlie DMA (Gierthmühlen et al.
2012), as it is the increased response of neurons to stroking stimuli. Intriguingly, some groups have offered a peripheral explanation for DMA (Liljencrantz et al.
2013), whereby an alteration in C-tactile afferent function, which typically mediates a pleasant percept associated with low force slow stroking touch, communicates noxious experience. This explanation then lends weight to research suggesting that an early mechanism behind ASD may be an alteration in CT fibre function (Cascio et al.
2018; Gordon et al.
2013; Kaiser et al.
2016; Walker and McGlone
2013). It is clear that this proposition requires further investigation. However, QST cannot fully distinguish between central and peripheral alterations (Mücke et al.
2014), therefore we can only speculate at this time.
There are striking similarities between our findings and those of Fründt et al. (
2017). Both were independently conducted, in parallel, and sought to use the DFNS QST protocol to identify differences that might exists in somatosensory function is ASD. Both studies found little evidence for a diagnosis-wide change in either somatosensory detection or pain thresholds. Both also found that when Z-scores were compared to published norms more autistic individuals showed a-typical data points, suggesting that individual differences may be of importance. This replication is particularly powerful as psychological sciences wrestle with the reproducibility crisis (Aarts et al.
2015). Here, independent verification of findings has been achieved, to provide a platform upon which to build future research.
An advantage of the standardised QST method is the published normative data which provides clear definitions of sensory loss and gain. The ASD phenotype can drastically differ and has large individual differences meaning the typical group analyses may not be advantageous to understanding this spectrum condition. Such published norms, which an individual’s QST pattern can be compared to, provides the opportunity to quantify individual cases. Individual analyses revealed a greater inter-individual variance with more Z-scores outside the 95% confidence interval of the DFNS published normative values in the ASD group (n = 32). This variance was present in all QST parameters and was not driven by a single participant (n = 13 participants). This might reflect the general heterogeneity of the ASD group; such heterogeneity belies the attempt to group this population under one diagnostic umbrella. The utility of this type of analysis is best shown in Fig.
3, which illustrates the sensory profiles of autistic individuals, and their sensory changes (see
results section). This also allows individual differences in the phenotypic presentation of ASD to be considered alongside their QST pattern.
In order to gain a self-report measure of motivation for pain avoidance, individuals were asked: “how much would they like to avoid feeling the stimulus again?”. However, these results were inconclusive. Self-report measures of pain motivation do not appear therefore, to access motivation in a way that provides a clearer or deeper understanding. For this reason, elegant experimental paradigms that have been used in healthy populations for understanding goal attenuation of avoidance behaviour could be adopted and utilised in an ASD population (Claes et al.
2015,
2014; Meulders and Vlaeyen
2012,
2013). Such experiments can implicitly test motivation that goes beyond conscious self-reporting by measuring behavioural responses and understanding avoidance in the context of multiple goals. This could be of vital importance in a population driven to achieve their repetitive or restrictive behaviour patterns regardless of other incoming behaviourally motivational stimuli, such as pain. Furthermore, given that the QST battery revealed typical nerve fibre function and that CPM appeared typical, this approach may help to pull apart the altered pain behaviours by considering top down modulation of pain.
Given the nature of sensory testing- applying a stimulus and recording verbally the perception of that stimulus, the underlying mechanisms can only be judiciously speculated upon. The pain experience in such studies is delivered in controlled environments, devoid of motivational context or other environmental cues. This absence of environmental context, results in a lack of knowledge about how distraction and other psychological effects might affect pain perception in ASD or how they modulate the more simple sensory experience of an input. It is also understandable, brief and cutaneous in nature, which may not reflect the diversity of pain in the real world (the relative merits and challenges of QST measures have been considered extensively elsewhere e.g. Backonja et al.
2013; Rolke et al.
2006). By comparison, naturally occurring pain is frequently endogenous, of longer duration, can be diffuse, and typically involves multiple pain systems. Further, ethical standards of pain induction that mitigate the threat of pain, fundamentally altering the emotional and motivational significance of pain is arguably a key feature of pain that emerges naturally (Edens and Gil
1995). The cost of such control is the potential lack of relevance to naturally occurring pain (Robertson and Low
2006; Rollman
2005). The methodological challenge is to develop techniques that combine the benefits of laboratory control with the relevance of pain that emerges naturally (Moore et al.
2013).
The findings of the present study should be considered in light of several limitations; notably the small sample size, which is common in the literature (Cascio et al.
2008; Duerden et al.
2015; Fründt et al.
2017; Guclu et al.
2007). Many autistic individuals find novel environments distressing and therefore may be unlikely to participate. Additionally, fear of pain and anxiety may likely reduce participation in experimental pain research (Karos et al.
2018). This paired with an exclusion of those with anxiety and depression, placed further limitations on recruitment numbers. This control, however, gives added validity to the results, as these conditions are known to have effects on pain perception (for review see Goesling et al.
2013; Thompson et al.
2016). Future studies should adopt this singular diagnosis approach and increase sample size, regardless of the difficulties caused by frequent psychiatric comorbidities in this population (Joshi et al.
2013). A related limitation is the inability to examine the effect of individual differences on pain responses, specifically IQ. Although participants had been formally assessed for a diagnosis of ASD and had been assessed for IQ in the normal range by a trained clinician, it was not possible to obtain detailed psychometrics. Further independent testing of IQ within the study, was deemed to be burdensome and in the interests of the well-being of the participant, was excluded from the protocol. Additionally, the addition of an IQ test to an already extensive protocol may have increased stress and therefore resulted in an unrepresentative response to stimuli. It would be beneficial in future studies to find mechanisms to understand key individual differences which might affect pain response in ASD. IQ in particular may be an important factor to consider as it has been shown that thermal pain response may be correlated with IQ, with participant’s with a lower IQ score having higher thresholds (Duerden et al.
2015). It was not possible to test this finding in the current research.
In conclusion, there was no systematic alteration to suggest an underlying dysfunction in the cutaneous somatosensory modalities tested in this study. There was a larger number of outlying z-score values within the ASD group. Further, dynamic mechanical allodynia and paradoxical heat sensations were present in some ASD participants, which is typically only observed in patients with peripheral neuropathy. Central processing and integration of sensory information rather than peripheral perception seems to be a better candidate for further research within ASD. In order to test this theory, future studies should focus on combining QST measurements with neuroimaging to detect probable processing differences. Additionally, studies could use experimental paradigms that test pain motivation to assess top-down modulation as a potential cause of altered pain behaviours in this population.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.