Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2017

17-08-2016 | Original Article

A drop in performance on a fluid intelligence test due to instructed-rule mindset

Auteurs: Hadas ErEl, Nachshon Meiran

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A ‘mindset’ is a configuration of processing resources that are made available for the task at hand as well as their suitable tuning for carrying it out. Of special interest, remote-relation abstract mindsets are introduced by activities sharing only general control processes with the task. To test the effect of a remote-relation mindset on performance on a Fluid Intelligence test (Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices, RAPM), we induced a mindset associated with little usage of executive processing by requiring participants to execute a well-defined classification rule 12 times, a manipulation known from previous work to drastically impair rule-generation performance and associated cognitive processes. In Experiment 1, this manipulation led to a drop in RAPM performance equivalent to 10.1 IQ points. No drop was observed in a General Knowledge task. In Experiment 2, a similar drop in RAPM performance was observed (equivalent to 7.9 and 9.2 IQ points) regardless if participants were pre-informed about the upcoming RAPM test. These results indicate strong (most likely, transient) adverse effects of a remote-relation mindset on test performance. They imply that although the trait of Fluid Intelligence has probably not changed, mindsets can severely distort estimates of this trait.
Literatuur
go back to reference Aronson, J., & McGlone, M. S. (2008). Stereotype and social identity threat. In T. Nelson (Ed.), The handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 153–178). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Aronson, J., & McGlone, M. S. (2008). Stereotype and social identity threat. In T. Nelson (Ed.), The handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 153–178). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on african american college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1491.CrossRef Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on african american college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125. doi:10.​1006/​jesp.​2001.​1491.CrossRef
go back to reference Ashby, F. G., & Isen, A. M. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.CrossRefPubMed Ashby, F. G., & Isen, A. M. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bors, D. A., & Stokes, T. L. (1998). Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices: norms for first-year university students and the development of a short form. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 382–398. doi:10.1177/0013164498058003002.CrossRef Bors, D. A., & Stokes, T. L. (1998). Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices: norms for first-year university students and the development of a short form. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 382–398. doi:10.​1177/​0013164498058003​002.CrossRef
go back to reference Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: its structure, growth and action. Oxford: North-Holland. Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: its structure, growth and action. Oxford: North-Holland.
go back to reference Colzato, L. S., van der Wel, P., Sellaro, R., & Hommel, B. (2016). A single bout of meditation biases cognitive control but not attentional focusing: evidence from the global–local task. Consciousness and Cognition, 39, 1–7.CrossRefPubMed Colzato, L. S., van der Wel, P., Sellaro, R., & Hommel, B. (2016). A single bout of meditation biases cognitive control but not attentional focusing: evidence from the global–local task. Consciousness and Cognition, 39, 1–7.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–184. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00096-4.CrossRef Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–184. doi:10.​1016/​S0160-2896(01)00096-4.CrossRef
go back to reference Dreisbach, G. (2012). Mechanisms of cognitive control the functional role of task rules. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 227–231.CrossRef Dreisbach, G. (2012). Mechanisms of cognitive control the functional role of task rules. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 227–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353.CrossRefPubMed Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dreisbach, G., & Haider, H. (2008). That’s what task sets are for: shielding against irrelevant information. Psychological Research, 72, 355–361.CrossRefPubMed Dreisbach, G., & Haider, H. (2008). That’s what task sets are for: shielding against irrelevant information. Psychological Research, 72, 355–361.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dreisbach, G., & Haider, H. (2009). How task representations guide attention: further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 477–486. doi:10.1037/a0014647.CrossRefPubMed Dreisbach, G., & Haider, H. (2009). How task representations guide attention: further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 477–486. doi:10.​1037/​a0014647.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2011). Role of test motivation in intelligence testing. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 7716–7720. doi:10.1073/pnas.1018601108.CrossRef Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2011). Role of test motivation in intelligence testing. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 7716–7720. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​1018601108.CrossRef
go back to reference Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 739–752. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.003.CrossRef Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 739–752. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jesp.​2004.​04.​003.CrossRef
go back to reference Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological science, 17, 172–179.CrossRef Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological science, 17, 172–179.CrossRef
go back to reference Galinsky, A. D., & Kray, L. J. (2004). From thinking about what might have been to sharing what we know: the effects of counterfactual mind-sets on information sharing in groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 606–618. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.005.CrossRef Galinsky, A. D., & Kray, L. J. (2004). From thinking about what might have been to sharing what we know: the effects of counterfactual mind-sets on information sharing in groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 606–618. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jesp.​2003.​11.​005.CrossRef
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Mindset theory of action phases. In P. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 526–545). London: Sage. Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Mindset theory of action phases. In P. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 526–545). London: Sage.
go back to reference Goschke, T. (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Eds.), Voluntary action. An issue at the interface of nature and culture (pp. 49–85). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Goschke, T. (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Eds.), Voluntary action. An issue at the interface of nature and culture (pp. 49–85). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Grange, J. A., & Houghton, G. (Eds.). (2014). Task switching and cognitive control. New York: Oxford University Press. Grange, J. A., & Houghton, G. (Eds.). (2014). Task switching and cognitive control. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Heitz, R. P., Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory capacity, attention control, and fluid intelligence. In O. Wilhelm R. W. Engle (Ed.), Understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 61–77). New York, NY: Sage Publications, Inc. doi:10.4135/9781452233529.n5. Heitz, R. P., Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory capacity, attention control, and fluid intelligence. In O. Wilhelm R. W. Engle (Ed.), Understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 61–77). New York, NY: Sage Publications, Inc. doi:10.​4135/​9781452233529.​n5.
go back to reference Inzlicht, M., & Schmader, T. (2011). Stereotype threat: Theory, process, and application. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Inzlicht, M., & Schmader, T. (2011). Stereotype threat: Theory, process, and application. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jeffreys, H. (Ed.). (1939/1961). The theory of probability (1st/3rd ed.) Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. Jeffreys, H. (Ed.). (1939/1961). The theory of probability (1st/3rd ed.) Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
go back to reference Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 8871–8876. doi:10.1073/pnas.1002007107.CrossRef Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 8871–8876. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​1002007107.CrossRef
go back to reference Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 131–141.CrossRef Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 131–141.CrossRef
go back to reference Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682. doi:10.1037/a0020198.CrossRef Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682. doi:10.​1037/​a0020198.CrossRef
go back to reference Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The prepared mind: neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17, 882–890. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01798.x.CrossRefPubMed Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The prepared mind: neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17, 882–890. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1467-9280.​2006.​01798.​x.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Love, J., Selker, R., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Verhagen, A. J., Ly, A.,… Wagenmakers, E. J. (2015). JASP (version 0.6.6) [computer software]. Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org. Love, J., Selker, R., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Verhagen, A. J., Ly, A.,… Wagenmakers, E. J. (2015). JASP (version 0.6.6) [computer software]. Retrieved from https://​jasp-stats.​org.
go back to reference Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54, i-95. doi:10.1037/h0093502. Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54, i-95. doi:10.​1037/​h0093502.
go back to reference Marshalek, B., Lohman, D. F., & Snow, R. E. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence, 7, 107–127CrossRef Marshalek, B., Lohman, D. F., & Snow, R. E. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence, 7, 107–127CrossRef
go back to reference Markman, K. D., Lindberg, M. J., Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2007). Implications of counterfactual structure for creative generation and analytical problem solving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 312–324. doi:10.1177/0146167206296106.CrossRefPubMed Markman, K. D., Lindberg, M. J., Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2007). Implications of counterfactual structure for creative generation and analytical problem solving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 312–324. doi:10.​1177/​0146167206296106​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nguyen, H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334. doi:10.1037/a0012702.CrossRefPubMed Nguyen, H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334. doi:10.​1037/​a0012702.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012a). Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin. American Psychologist, 67, 503–504. doi:10.1037/a0029772.CrossRefPubMed Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012a). Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin. American Psychologist, 67, 503–504. doi:10.​1037/​a0029772.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012b). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699.CrossRefPubMed Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012b). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159. doi:10.​1037/​a0026699.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary scales Oxford. England: Oxford Psychologists Press. Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary scales Oxford. England: Oxford Psychologists Press.
go back to reference Reisenauer, R., & Dreisbach, G. (2013). The impact of task rules on distracter processing: automatic categorization of irrelevant stimuli. Psychological Research, 77, 128–138.CrossRefPubMed Reisenauer, R., & Dreisbach, G. (2013). The impact of task rules on distracter processing: automatic categorization of irrelevant stimuli. Psychological Research, 77, 128–138.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Steele, C. M. (1998). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. In J. L. Eberhardt S. & T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and the response (pp. 202-233). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Steele, C. M. (1998). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. In J. L. Eberhardt S. & T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and the response (pp. 202-233). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Metagegevens
Titel
A drop in performance on a fluid intelligence test due to instructed-rule mindset
Auteurs
Hadas ErEl
Nachshon Meiran
Publicatiedatum
17-08-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0796-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2017

Psychological Research 5/2017 Naar de uitgave