Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 4/2021

05-01-2021 | Original Article

A Dissociation Between Two Cognitive Biases in Acute Stress Disorder: Preliminary Evidence for the Reverse Gambling Fallacy

Auteurs: Yaakov S. G. Hoffman, Yaira Hamama-Raz, Menachem Ben-Ezra, Osnat Lavenda

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Uitgave 4/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Background

Rationality biases, such as the gambling fallacy (e.g., predicting future coin-tosses based on previous tosses) and the famous “Linda” conjunction fallacy (estimating the conjunction that “Linda” is both teller and feminist based on her description) have not been examined in people suffering from acute stress disorder (ASD). We analyze potential outcomes and align them with different theories.

Methods

To discern the precise pattern of rationality biases in persons with ASD, we examined performance on these 2 tasks within a month of the Hayian Super-Typhoon (August 27th, 2013). Out of a sample of 1001 persons, 82 had clinical ASD and their performance was compared to the remaining 919 participants.

Results

A specific link between ASD and rationality biases revealed that although conjunction task performance was not associated with ASD diagnosis, coin-task performance was. Namely, responding “Heads” to a 6th coin-toss after 5 successive “Heads” (reverse gambling fallacy) was robustly linked with ASD diagnosis.

Conclusion

The results align with the bridging of trauma theories claiming that trauma symptoms are generated by disequilibrium following trauma exposure, with prospect theory’s notion of chance, which is conceived as belief in equilibrium restoration. Such disequilibrium following trauma exposure is thus linked with the belief underlying reverse gambling fallacy biases, namely “what-was-will-be”. Implications regarding themes important to address in therapy are mentioned.
Voetnoten
1
These follow up analyses yielded the same pattern for all types of post-hoc in the SPSS 25 package (e.g., Scheffe, Bonferroni, Tukey etc.).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
go back to reference Ayton, P., & Fischer, I. (2004). The hot hand fallacy and the gambler’s fallacy: Two faces of subjective randomness? Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 1369–1378.CrossRef Ayton, P., & Fischer, I. (2004). The hot hand fallacy and the gambler’s fallacy: Two faces of subjective randomness? Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 1369–1378.CrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(2), 234–238.PubMedCrossRef Baddeley, A. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(2), 234–238.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bard, M., & Sangrey, D. (1979). The crime Victim's book. New York: Basic Books. Bard, M., & Sangrey, D. (1979). The crime Victim's book. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Ben-Ezra, M., Leshem, E., & Goodwin, R. (2015). In the wake of national trauma: Psychological reactions following the Charlie Hebdo terror attack. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(8), 795–796.CrossRefPubMed Ben-Ezra, M., Leshem, E., & Goodwin, R. (2015). In the wake of national trauma: Psychological reactions following the Charlie Hebdo terror attack. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(8), 795–796.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ben-Ezra, M., Hamama-Raz, Y., Mahat-Shamir, M., Pitcho-Prelorentzos, S., & Kaniasty, K. (2017). Shattering core beliefs: Psychological reactions to mass shooting in Orlando. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 85, 56–58.PubMedCrossRef Ben-Ezra, M., Hamama-Raz, Y., Mahat-Shamir, M., Pitcho-Prelorentzos, S., & Kaniasty, K. (2017). Shattering core beliefs: Psychological reactions to mass shooting in Orlando. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 85, 56–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bensi, L., & Giusberti, F. (2007). Trait anxiety and reasoning under uncertainty. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 827–838.CrossRef Bensi, L., & Giusberti, F. (2007). Trait anxiety and reasoning under uncertainty. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 827–838.CrossRef
go back to reference Botsford, J., Steinbrink, M., Rimane, E., Rosner, R., Steil, R., & Renneberg, B. (2019). Maladaptive post-traumatic cognitions in interpersonally traumatized adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder: An analysis of “stuck-points”. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(1), 284–294.CrossRef Botsford, J., Steinbrink, M., Rimane, E., Rosner, R., Steil, R., & Renneberg, B. (2019). Maladaptive post-traumatic cognitions in interpersonally traumatized adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder: An analysis of “stuck-points”. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(1), 284–294.CrossRef
go back to reference Bryant, R. A., Creamer, M., O'Donnell, M., Silove, D., McFarlane, A. C., & Forbes, D. (2015). A comparison of the capacity of DSM-IV and DSM-5 acute stress disorder definitions to predict posttraumatic stress disorder and related disorders. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 76(4), 391–397.PubMedCrossRef Bryant, R. A., Creamer, M., O'Donnell, M., Silove, D., McFarlane, A. C., & Forbes, D. (2015). A comparison of the capacity of DSM-IV and DSM-5 acute stress disorder definitions to predict posttraumatic stress disorder and related disorders. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 76(4), 391–397.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Croskerry, P., Singhal, G., & Mamede, S. (2013). Cognitive debiasing 1: Origins of bias and theory of debiasing. BMJ Quality & Safety, 22, ii58–ii64.CrossRef Croskerry, P., Singhal, G., & Mamede, S. (2013). Cognitive debiasing 1: Origins of bias and theory of debiasing. BMJ Quality & Safety, 22, ii58–ii64.CrossRef
go back to reference Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2010). Introduction to the special issue: Emotional states, attention, and working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 189–199.CrossRef Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2010). Introduction to the special issue: Emotional states, attention, and working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 189–199.CrossRef
go back to reference Epstein, S., Denes-Raj, V., & Pacini, R. (1995). The Linda problem revisited from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1124–1138.CrossRef Epstein, S., Denes-Raj, V., & Pacini, R. (1995). The Linda problem revisited from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1124–1138.CrossRef
go back to reference Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive–experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405.PubMedCrossRef Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive–experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eysenck, M. W., Derekeshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–352.PubMedCrossRef Eysenck, M. W., Derekeshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–352.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Feeney, A., Shafto, P., & Dunning, D. (2007). Who is susceptible to conjunction fallacies in category-based induction? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 884–889.CrossRef Feeney, A., Shafto, P., & Dunning, D. (2007). Who is susceptible to conjunction fallacies in category-based induction? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 884–889.CrossRef
go back to reference Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1989). Behavioral/cognitive conceptualizations of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20(2), 155–176.CrossRef Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1989). Behavioral/cognitive conceptualizations of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20(2), 155–176.CrossRef
go back to reference Frankl, V. E. (1969). The will to meaning. New York: New American Library. Frankl, V. E. (1969). The will to meaning. New York: New American Library.
go back to reference Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. (2003). The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey of mental health and well-being. Psychological Medicine, 33(2), 357–362.PubMedCrossRef Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. (2003). The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey of mental health and well-being. Psychological Medicine, 33(2), 357–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gerhart, J. I., Canetti, D., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2015). Traumatic stress in overview: Definition, context, scope, and long-term outcomes. In Traumatic stress and long-term recovery (pp. 3–24). Cham: Springer.CrossRef Gerhart, J. I., Canetti, D., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2015). Traumatic stress in overview: Definition, context, scope, and long-term outcomes. In Traumatic stress and long-term recovery (pp. 3–24). Cham: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 295–314.CrossRef Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 295–314.CrossRef
go back to reference Grossman, E. S., Hoffman, Y. S., Berger, I., & Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2015). Beating their chests: University students with ADHD demonstrate greater attentional abilities on an inattentional blindness paradigm. Neuropsychology, 29(6), 882–887.PubMedCrossRef Grossman, E. S., Hoffman, Y. S., Berger, I., & Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2015). Beating their chests: University students with ADHD demonstrate greater attentional abilities on an inattentional blindness paradigm. Neuropsychology, 29(6), 882–887.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hamama-Raz, Y., Palgi, Y., Leshem, E., Ben-Ezra, M., & Lavenda, O. (2017). Typhoon survivors' subjective wellbeing—A different view of responses to natural disaster. PLoS One, 12(9), e0184327.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hamama-Raz, Y., Palgi, Y., Leshem, E., Ben-Ezra, M., & Lavenda, O. (2017). Typhoon survivors' subjective wellbeing—A different view of responses to natural disaster. PLoS One, 12(9), e0184327.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A prospective evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(3), 507–512.PubMedCrossRef Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A prospective evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(3), 507–512.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoe, S. L. (2008). Issues and procedures in adopting structural equation modeling technique. Journal of Applied Quantitative Methods, 3, 76–83. Hoe, S. L. (2008). Issues and procedures in adopting structural equation modeling technique. Journal of Applied Quantitative Methods, 3, 76–83.
go back to reference Hoffman, Y., Shrira, A., Bodner, E., & Ben-Ezra, M. (2017). Prime and prejudice: The effect of priming context and prejudicial attitudes on post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms following immigrant violence. Psychiatry Research, 254, 224–231.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, Y., Shrira, A., Bodner, E., & Ben-Ezra, M. (2017). Prime and prejudice: The effect of priming context and prejudicial attitudes on post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms following immigrant violence. Psychiatry Research, 254, 224–231.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Horowitz, M. J. (2011). Stress response syndromes: PTSD, grief, adjustment, and dissociative disorders. Jason Aronson, Incorporated. Horowitz, M. J. (2011). Stress response syndromes: PTSD, grief, adjustment, and dissociative disorders. Jason Aronson, Incorporated.
go back to reference Horowitz, M. J. (2015). Effects of trauma on sense of self. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 20(2), 189–193.CrossRef Horowitz, M. J. (2015). Effects of trauma on sense of self. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 20(2), 189–193.CrossRef
go back to reference Huber, J., Kirchler, M., & Stöckl, T. (2010). The hot hand belief and the gambler’s fallacy in investment decisions under risk. Theory and Decision, 68(4), 445–462.CrossRef Huber, J., Kirchler, M., & Stöckl, T. (2010). The hot hand belief and the gambler’s fallacy in investment decisions under risk. Theory and Decision, 68(4), 445–462.CrossRef
go back to reference Janoff-Bulman, R. (1985). The aftermath of victimization: Rebuilding shattered assumptions. Trauma and its wake, 1, 15–35. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1985). The aftermath of victimization: Rebuilding shattered assumptions. Trauma and its wake, 1, 15–35.
go back to reference Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Our fundamental assumptions: Shattered assumptions. The free press. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Our fundamental assumptions: Shattered assumptions. The free press.
go back to reference Janoff-Bulman, R. (1999). Rebuilding shattered assumptions after traumatic life events. Coping: The psychology of what works, 305–323. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1999). Rebuilding shattered assumptions after traumatic life events. Coping: The psychology of what works, 305–323.
go back to reference Janoff-Bulman, R. (2010). Shattered assumptions. Simon and Schuster. Janoff-Bulman, R. (2010). Shattered assumptions. Simon and Schuster.
go back to reference Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.CrossRefPubMed Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454.CrossRef Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454.CrossRef
go back to reference Kessler, R. C., Barker, P. R., Colpe, L. J., Epstein, J. F., Gfroerer, J. C., Hiripi, E., et al. (2003). Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(2), 184–189.PubMedCrossRef Kessler, R. C., Barker, P. R., Colpe, L. J., Epstein, J. F., Gfroerer, J. C., Hiripi, E., et al. (2003). Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(2), 184–189.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kleim, B., Ehlers, A., & Glucksman, E. (2012a). Investigating cognitive pathways to psychopathology: Predicting depression and posttraumatic stress disorder from early responses after assault. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(5), –527. Kleim, B., Ehlers, A., & Glucksman, E. (2012a). Investigating cognitive pathways to psychopathology: Predicting depression and posttraumatic stress disorder from early responses after assault. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(5), –527.
go back to reference Kleim, B., Ehring, T., & Ehlers, A. (2012b). Perceptual processing advantages for trauma-related visual cues in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychological Medicine, 42(1), 173–181.PubMedCrossRef Kleim, B., Ehring, T., & Ehlers, A. (2012b). Perceptual processing advantages for trauma-related visual cues in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychological Medicine, 42(1), 173–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lavenda, O., Grossman, E. S., Ben-Ezra, M., & Hoffman, Y. (2017). Exploring DSM-5 criterion a in acute stress disorder symptoms following natural disaster. Psychiatry Research, 256, 458–460.PubMedCrossRef Lavenda, O., Grossman, E. S., Ben-Ezra, M., & Hoffman, Y. (2017). Exploring DSM-5 criterion a in acute stress disorder symptoms following natural disaster. Psychiatry Research, 256, 458–460.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leon, M. R., & Revelle, W. (1985). Effects of anxiety on analogical reasoning: A test of three theoretical models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(5), 1302–1315.PubMedCrossRef Leon, M. R., & Revelle, W. (1985). Effects of anxiety on analogical reasoning: A test of three theoretical models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(5), 1302–1315.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liberali, J. M., Reyna, V. F., Furlan, S., Stein, L. M., & Pardo, S. T. (2012). Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 361–381.PubMedCrossRef Liberali, J. M., Reyna, V. F., Furlan, S., Stein, L. M., & Pardo, S. T. (2012). Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 361–381.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacLaren, V., Ellery, M., & Knoll, T. (2015). Personality, gambling motives and cognitive distortions in electronic gambling machine players. Personality and Individual Differences, 73, 24–28.CrossRef MacLaren, V., Ellery, M., & Knoll, T. (2015). Personality, gambling motives and cognitive distortions in electronic gambling machine players. Personality and Individual Differences, 73, 24–28.CrossRef
go back to reference McNally, R. J., & Foa, E. B. (1987). Cognition and agoraphobia: Bias in the interpretation of threat. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11(5), 567–581.CrossRef McNally, R. J., & Foa, E. B. (1987). Cognition and agoraphobia: Bias in the interpretation of threat. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11(5), 567–581.CrossRef
go back to reference Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). Danger expectancies and insight in acrophobia. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33(2), 215–221.CrossRef Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). Danger expectancies and insight in acrophobia. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33(2), 215–221.CrossRef
go back to reference Michael, T., Ehlers, A., & Halligan, S. L. (2005). Enhanced priming for trauma-related material in posttraumatic stress disorder. Emotion, 5(1), 103–112.PubMedCrossRef Michael, T., Ehlers, A., & Halligan, S. L. (2005). Enhanced priming for trauma-related material in posttraumatic stress disorder. Emotion, 5(1), 103–112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Park, C. L., Riley, K. E., George, L. S., Gutierrez, I. A., Hale, A. E., Cho, D., & Braun, T. D. (2016). Assessing disruptions in meaning: Development of the global meaning violation scale. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 40(6), 831–846.CrossRef Park, C. L., Riley, K. E., George, L. S., Gutierrez, I. A., Hale, A. E., Cho, D., & Braun, T. D. (2016). Assessing disruptions in meaning: Development of the global meaning violation scale. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 40(6), 831–846.CrossRef
go back to reference Pelster, M. (2020). The gambler’s and hot-hand fallacies: Empirical evidence from trading data. Economics Letters, 187, 108887.CrossRef Pelster, M. (2020). The gambler’s and hot-hand fallacies: Empirical evidence from trading data. Economics Letters, 187, 108887.CrossRef
go back to reference Roney, C. J., & Trick, L. M. (2003). Grouping and gambling: A gestalt approach to understanding the gambler's fallacy. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 57(2), 69–75.PubMedCrossRef Roney, C. J., & Trick, L. M. (2003). Grouping and gambling: A gestalt approach to understanding the gambler's fallacy. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 57(2), 69–75.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roney, C. J., & Trick, L. M. (2009). Sympathetic magic and perceptions of randomness: The hot hand versus the gambler's fallacy. Thinking & Reasoning, 15(2), 197–210.CrossRef Roney, C. J., & Trick, L. M. (2009). Sympathetic magic and perceptions of randomness: The hot hand versus the gambler's fallacy. Thinking & Reasoning, 15(2), 197–210.CrossRef
go back to reference Rothbaum, B. O., & Davis, M. (2003). Applying learning principles to the treatment of post-trauma reactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008(1), 112–121.PubMedCrossRef Rothbaum, B. O., & Davis, M. (2003). Applying learning principles to the treatment of post-trauma reactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008(1), 112–121.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2000). The generality of cognitive bias in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(7), 709–715.PubMedCrossRef Smith, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2000). The generality of cognitive bias in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(7), 709–715.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Staab, J. P., Grieger, T. A., Fullerton, C. S., & Ursano, R. J. (1996). Acute stress disorder, subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder and depression after a series of typhoons. Anxiety, 2(5), 219–225.PubMedCrossRef Staab, J. P., Grieger, T. A., Fullerton, C. S., & Ursano, R. J. (1996). Acute stress disorder, subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder and depression after a series of typhoons. Anxiety, 2(5), 219–225.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stanovich, K., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Thinking & Reasoning, 4(4), 289–317.CrossRef Stanovich, K., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Thinking & Reasoning, 4(4), 289–317.CrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76(2), 105–110.CrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76(2), 105–110.CrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.PubMedCrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extension versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review., 90(4), 293–315.CrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extension versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review., 90(4), 293–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Warda, G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). Cognitive bias in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(12), 1177–1183.PubMedCrossRef Warda, G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). Cognitive bias in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(12), 1177–1183.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference West, R. F., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: Associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 930–941.CrossRef West, R. F., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: Associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 930–941.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
A Dissociation Between Two Cognitive Biases in Acute Stress Disorder: Preliminary Evidence for the Reverse Gambling Fallacy
Auteurs
Yaakov S. G. Hoffman
Yaira Hamama-Raz
Menachem Ben-Ezra
Osnat Lavenda
Publicatiedatum
05-01-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Uitgave 4/2021
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10196-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2021

Cognitive Therapy and Research 4/2021 Naar de uitgave