Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 August 2021
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic The Role of Emotional Granularity in Emotional Regulation, Mental Disorders, and Well-being View all 13 articles

Negative Emotion Differentiation Predicts Psychotherapy Outcome: Preliminary Findings

  • Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States

Emotion differentiation (ED), the extent to which same-valenced emotions are experienced as distinct, is considered a valuable ability in various contexts owing to the essential affect-related information it provides. This information can help individuals understand and regulate their emotional and motivational states. In this study, we sought to examine the extent to which ED can be beneficial in psychotherapy context and specifically for predicting treatment response. Thirty-two prospective patients with mood and anxiety disorders completed four daily assessments of negative and positive emotions for 30 days before receiving cognitive-behavioral treatment. Depression, stress, and anxiety symptoms severity were assessed pre- and post-treatment using self-reports and clinical interviews. We conducted a series of hierarchical regression models in which symptoms change scores were predicted by ED while adjusting for the mean and variability. We found that negative ED was associated with greater self-reported treatment response (except for anxiety) when negative emotional variability (EV) was included in the models. Probing negative ED and EV’s interactive effects suggested that negative ED was associated with greater treatment response (except for anxiety) for individuals with lower EV levels. Results were obtained while controlling for mean negative affect. Our findings suggest that negative ED can benefit psychotherapy patients whose negative emotions are relatively less variable. We discuss the meaning of suppression and interactive effects between affect dynamics and consider possible clinical implications.

Introduction

Emotion differentiation (ED), the extent to which emotions are experienced (and labeled) as distinct, has been found to be associated with various positive outcomes (for a meta-analysis, see Seah and Coifman, 2021). It is considered a valuable ability in multiple contexts, providing individuals with essential affect-related information that can guide their behavior in an adaptive manner (Schwarz, 2012; Kashdan et al., 2015). The present study set out to examine whether ED can be beneficial in the context of psychotherapy, and specifically, to what extent those with greater ED respond better to a personalized cognitive-behavioral treatment. We first review findings tying emotion-related constructs to psychotherapy response and then note limitations with their current operationalizations. Subsequently, we explain how ED, obtained using dynamic assessment, can function as a promising predictor of such response.

Patients vary significantly in their response to psychotherapeutic interventions (e.g., Lambert, 2013; Boswell et al., 2016). Traditionally, this variance has been attributed to three classes of factors: treatment factors (Marcus et al., 2014; Firth et al., 2019; e.g., technique), therapist factors (Baldwin and Imel, 2013; e.g., experience), and patient factors (Bohart and Wade, 2013; e.g., personality traits). Among these classes, leading researchers (e.g., Wampold, 2010; Norcross and Lambert, 2011) estimate that a large portion of the treatment response variance can be explained by patient factors, that is, by pre-existing individual differences between patients.

Identifying which patients are likely to respond poorly to treatment and which factors underlie this response can have important clinical implications regarding treatment planning. Such factors can sometimes be addressed by therapists employing specific psychotherapeutic interventions. Moreover, these factors can inform caregivers regarding the intensity of the recommended treatment or indicate a need to employ other treatment modalities (e.g., group psychotherapy and psychopharmacology).

For many years (e.g., Luborsky et al., 1971), clinicians and researchers have attempted to discover specific patient characteristics that are predictive of therapeutic improvement (for review, see Bohart and Wade, 2013). Some characteristics, such as demographic variables (e.g., gender or age), have failed to show consistent associations with therapy outcomes (Cuijpers et al., 2009; Bohart and Wade, 2013). Other characteristics, such as symptom severity (e.g., Firth et al., 2019) or patients’ therapy-related expectancies and motivational factors (e.g., Newman et al., 2006; Constantino et al., 2011), have been identified as more consistent predictors.

Emotional experience, expression, and regulation have all been proposed as key patient factors that can affect psychotherapy outcome (e.g., Greenberg and Safran, 1989; Thoma and McKay, 2015; Fisher et al., 2016), with theoretical and empirical work pointing to the importance of monitoring, processing, and regulating emotions as integral psychotherapeutic process factors (e.g., Pascual-Leone and Greenberg, 2007; Pinna et al., 2020). Though compromised to some extent in many psychopathological conditions (e.g., Dryman and Heimberg, 2018), these abilities are considered vital for patients to be able to benefit from the psychotherapeutic process (e.g., Watson et al., 2011). Specifically, they allow for deeper examination and reflection over one’s experience, creating new meanings, and personally meaningful problem resolution (e.g., Watson and Bedard, 2006; Aafjes-van Doorn and Barber, 2017). In line with these notions, the quality of patients’ emotional processing during sessions was found to be tied to improved treatment outcomes (Pos et al., 2009; Aafjes-van Doorn and Barber, 2017; Pascual-Leone and Yeryomenko, 2017).

Whereas session-based emotional processing and regulation have been shown to be robust predictors of therapy outcome, the predictive validity of pre-treatment emotional processing or regulation indices have been less consistent. In particular, a recent systematic review by Pinna et al. (2020, p. 1) indicated that the links between self-reported alexithymia, an inability to identify and communicate emotions, and treatment outcomes are “complex.” Though in some studies alexithymia was tied to poorer treatment response (e.g., Quilty et al., 2017), this association was absent in others (e.g., Spek et al., 2008).

Additional work has examined other emotional processing or regulation variables as outcome predictors. For example, patients lower in pre-treatment emotion suppression had more favorable treatment outcomes (Scherer et al., 2017; Hosogoshi et al., 2020). Interestingly, in both studies, emotion reappraisal was not predictive of treatment outcome. Lastly, a measure of psychological mindedness, defined as the tendency to turn inward seeking psychological explanations of behavior, people, and problems, has provided mixed results as a predictor of therapy outcome (Bohart and Wade, 2013).

A common limitation shared by most studies addressing the links between patients’ abilities to express, process, and regulate their emotions and psychotherapy outcomes is their attempt to capture dynamic processes using a single-time static intake measurement (Fisher, 2015). This discrepancy severely hinders researchers’ ability to tap the processes underlying patients’ emotional difficulties accurately. The reliance on self-reports for items requiring a high level of reflective capacity further limits the measurement validity.

Though single-time clinician assessment is still considered the gold standard in psychotherapy practice and research, pre-treatment ecological momentary assessment (EMA) is increasingly being employed to assess clinically relevant factors (e.g., Lutz et al., 2018; Rubel et al., 2018; Fisher et al., 2019; Shalom et al., 2020). It allows for intensive repeated measurement of variables of interest and modeling their dynamic inter-relationships (e.g., Fisher and Boswell, 2016). Researchers and clinicians have started to capitalize on EMA’s strengths to explore the extent to which dynamic indices can inform treatment processes and outcomes (e.g., Husen et al., 2016; Lutz et al., 2018; Fisher et al., 2019).

Sophisticated analytic methods can be used to translate such dynamic indices into personalized treatment plans (Fisher and Boswell, 2016; Fisher et al., 2019; Wright and Woods, 2020). At the same time, simpler methods can help identify meaningful individual differences without prescribing specific interventions (e.g., Husen et al., 2016; Lutz et al., 2018; Bosley et al., 2019). Individual differences in the dynamic unfolding of affect (Kuppens et al., 2010) are particularly appealing and relevant in the context of psychotherapy (e.g., Husen et al., 2016).

One affect dynamics index that may be highly informative regarding emotional processing and regulation is emotion differentiation (ED). ED is defined as the extent to which same-valenced emotions are experienced and labeled in a distinct or granular manner (Barrett et al., 2001). Individuals with greater ED tend to represent and describe their emotional states using specific terms (e.g., “enthusiastic,” “irritable,” or “tense”), rather than general or abstract terms (e.g., “good” and “bad”).

Differentiation, particularly between negative emotions, has been tied to various positive outcomes in numerous studies (for a review, see Kashdan et al., 2015; for a recent meta-analysis, see Seah and Coifman, 2021). For example, negative ED has been related to greater self-esteem, lower neuroticism, and less depressive feelings (Erbas et al., 2018; Willroth et al., 2020). Additionally, negative ED was found to serve as a protective factor in the face of various daily stressors (in a community sample; Starr et al., 2017) and of the adverse outcomes of ruminations (in clinical samples; Zaki et al., 2013; Seah et al., 2020). Interestingly, in a recent study (Liu et al., 2020), only a combination of negative and positive ED (but neither independently) moderated an association between trait rumination and increases in depression.

A few candidates have been identified as possible mechanisms underlying the benefits of (mostly) negative ED. ED provides nuanced information about one’s emotions which is likely to translate to more adaptive emotion regulation processes. Indeed, greater negative ED was found to be tied to greater effectiveness of negative emotion downregulation strategies (Kalokerinos et al., 2019). Moreover, affect labeling, the act of putting feelings into words, is widely regarded as a form of implicit emotion regulation technique (for review, see Torre and Lieberman, 2018), and high negative ED individuals are likely to be more accurate and thorough in labeling their emotions. Additionally, greater ED may clarify motivational processes and consequently render the allocation of attentional and behavioral resources more efficient (Kashdan et al., 2015). Lastly, greater ED may involve more accurate causal attributions that rely on better access to the origins of one’s emotional experience. When adverse events are followed by more differentiated and less global emotional states, there is a greater likelihood of identifying the cause one’s emotional response. Accurate attributions are likely to be less depressive, that is, less internal, global, and stable (Seligman et al., 1979).

Positive ED, in contrast, has not been tied consistently with adaptive outcomes (despite being moderately correlated with negative ED; Liu et al., 2020). It has been found to be associated with favorable outcomes only under specific circumstances, such as among participants with borderline personality features (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014) or sub-clinical eating disorders (Selby et al., 2014). In other studies (e.g., Barrett et al., 2001; Demiralp et al., 2012; Kashdan and Farmer, 2014; Willroth et al., 2020), such associations did not emerge, and often they are not being examined or reported.

The impact of ED may be most pronounced and visible in conditions where emotions and their processing play diverse and fundamental roles. Working with patients’ emotions has been identified as a cornerstone of the psychotherapeutic process across theoretical orientations and disorders (Barlow et al., 2011; Greenberg, 2012; Thoma and McKay, 2015). Whereas different orientations may have different foci and employ distinct techniques, they share a primary change mechanism—accessing patients’ emotions and modifying their underlying cognitive-affective mental structures. Patients’ ability to differentiate between their emotions, particularly their negative ones, is probably of great value for such processes.

The Present Study

The present study expanded recent work regarding the role of dynamic affective patterns (e.g., Husen et al., 2016; Bosley et al., 2019) in patients’ response to psychotherapy by examining the extent to which ED is predictive of treatment outcome. Using EMA conducted prior to a personalized modular cognitive-behavioral treatment (see Fisher et al., 2019) for individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) or major depressive disorder (MDD), we prospectively estimated patients’ negative and positive ED prior to therapy. These indices were then used to predict patients’ symptomatic improvement from pre- to post-therapy. This study is a secondary data analysis of Fisher et al. (2019) and likewise an extension of Bosley et al. (2019). The latter work examined affect dynamics as predictors of symptoms severity and pre- to post-treatment symptomatic change but did not address ED.

Maladaptive emotional processes are at the core of the development and maintenance of both GAD and MDD. Individuals with GAD often suffer from excessive negative affect that is poorly understood and maladaptively regulated through recurrent worrying (e.g., Mennin et al., 2002). Individuals with MDD often suffer from difficulties identifying emotions, tolerating and accepting negative emotions, and effectively regulating their emotions, and tend to employ maladaptive regulation techniques (e.g., rumination and suppression; for review, see Rottenberg, 2017). These deficiencies in emotional processing are likely to be involved in cognitive biases, such as maladaptive causal attribution processes that play a key role in depression (Peterson and Seligman, 1984).

Given patients’ diagnoses, the characteristics of the treatment, and the nature of ED, we hypothesized that those patients who are better at differentiating between their negative emotions would show greater symptomatic improvement. We also examined patients’ ability to differentiate between their positive emotions, but we did not expect it to have a similar predictive role for two reasons. First, the evidence for associations between positive ED and adaptive outcomes is much weaker than for negative ED. Second, the context of psychotherapy for depression and anxiety, in which patients work through and around their negative emotions, probably renders their differentiation more meaningful.

Specifically, higher negative ED patients are likely to be more capable of identifying their core maladaptive emotional processes, including inefficient attempts to regulate them (e.g., worrying and ruminating). Moreover, greater negative ED can help patients reinterpret the meaning of negative situations and change maladaptive causal attributions (i.e., internal, global, and stable) that are central to the maintenance of their depressive symptoms. Lastly, the ability of higher negative ED patients to engage in psychotherapy sessions in an emotionally effective manner and regulate their emotions during the sessions is likely to allow for a more focused and efficient therapeutic process.

Notably, following recent work demonstrating limited incremental predictive validity of affect dynamics indices beyond the mean and variability (Bos et al., 2019; Dejonckheere et al., 2019; Wendt et al., 2020), we included these indices in all models. Of note, Bosley et al. (2019) found that the mean and standard deviation (SD) of negative emotions were not associated with treatment response and that the variability of positive emotions was associated with more significant symptom reduction.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The current study utilized data from an open trial of personalized modular psychotherapy for depression and anxiety based on the unified protocol (UP; Barlow et al., 2011). In this trial, participants with GAD and MDD completed four daily self-report assessments of affect, behavior, and symptoms for 30-day period prior to treatment. Subsequently, they received psychotherapeutic interventions tailored to their symptom dynamics as assessed during the EMA. A full description of the procedures and outcomes can be found in Fisher et al. (2019).

Individuals with symptoms consistent with possible GAD or MDD diagnoses were recruited from the greater San Francisco Bay Area using referrals, flyers, and internet advertisements. One hundred and seventy-four potential participants passed a brief telephone screening and were invited to an in-person appointment. They underwent a structured clinical interview (the Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-5; ADIS-5, Brown and Barlow, 2014) to verify their diagnosis and assess symptoms’ severity. Inclusion criteria included a primary diagnosis of MDD or GAD, age of 18 to 65 years, and a mobile phone with web access. Exclusion criteria included a history of psychosis or mania, concurrent or recent (within the past 12 months) cognitive-behavioral treatment. Interrater reliabilities for diagnoses (based on video recordings of the structured clinical interviews) were high—GAD and MDD had kappa values of 0.68 and 0.84, and percent agreement of 95 and 92%, respectively.

Fifty-seven individuals (33%) met the inclusion and exclusion criteria, and of these, 40 began treatment. Seven participants withdrew from the study during treatment, and one participant did not complete a post-treatment assessment, leaving 32 participants in the present sample. As shown in Table S1 in the online supplementary material (OSM),1 no significant differences were found in demographics, pre-treatment symptoms, and affect variables between participants who completed treatment and post-treatment assessment and those who did not. Twenty of 32 participants in the final sample (62.5%) identified as female, and the average age was 37.9 years (SD = 14.3). Sixteen participants (50%) identified as White, nine (28.1%) identified as Asian, four (12.5%) identified as Latino/a, one (3.1%) identified as Black, and two (6.2%) selected “other.” Thirteen (41.6%) individuals were diagnosed with current primary GAD, 5 (15.6%) were diagnosed with current primary MDD, and 14 (43.8%) met the criteria for co-primary diagnoses of both GAD and MDD. Sixteen (50%) participants had at least one current comorbid disorder other than GAD or MDD; these comorbid diagnoses included social anxiety disorder (n = 10; 31.2%), specific phobia (n = 4; 12.5%), persistent depressive disorder (n = 3; 9.4%), agoraphobia (n = 2; 6.2%), and posttraumatic stress disorder (n = 1; 3.1%).

Measures

Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression

The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, 1960) assesses the severity of depressive symptomatology. It is a 13-item clinician-administered scale providing severity rating of each overarching symptom cluster on a scale from 0 (not present) to 4 (very severe/incapacitating). The HRSD’s internal consistency ranges from adequate to good (0.73–0.81; Steer et al., 1987; Moras et al., 1992). Its total score interrater reliabilities range from 0.78 to 0.82 (Steer et al., 1987; Moras et al., 1992). HRSD scores correlate strongly with self-report depression measures in clinical samples (Steer et al., 1983).

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales

The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS) is a 42-item self-report questionnaire comprised of three subscales (14 items each) developed to capture levels of anxiety, depression, and stress, as described by the tripartite model (Clark and Watson, 1991; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). The anxiety subscale evaluates hyper-arousal unique to some forms of anxiety, and the depression subscale evaluates anhedonia or low positive affect unique to depression. As noted by several researchers (e.g., Holmes and Newman, 2006; Campbell-Sills and Brown, 2010), the DASS stress subscale primarily evaluates tension and irritability prevalent among individuals suffering from GAD. Hence, to create a measure relevant to our entire sample, we combined the depression and stress subscales to be used as the main self-report outcome measure, and used the anxiety subscale as an additional outcome measure. Items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 to 3 (“did not apply to me at all” to “applied to me very much or most of the time”). In our sample, all three subscales were highly reliable (Cronbach’s alphas for the anxiety subscale were 0.84 and 0.78 for pre- and post-treatment, respectively, for the depression subscale were 0.94 and 0.94 for pre- and post-treatment, respectively, and for the stress subscale were 0.89 and 0.90 for pre- and post-treatment, respectively). The Cronbach’s alphas for the combined depression-stress measure were 0.91 and 0.89 for pre- and post-treatment, respectively.

Momentary Affect

For each EMA survey, participants rated their emotional experience over the preceding hours across the survey items using a 0–100 visual analog slider with the anchors “not at all” and “as much as possible.” The surveys included four positive affect items (positive, energetic, enthusiastic, and content) and seven negative affect items (angry, irritable, worthless/guilty, frightened/afraid, down/depressed, worried, and hopeless). Additional items not used for the present study consisted of various symptoms (i.e., loss of interest or pleasure, restless, difficulty concentrating, muscle tension, fatigued, dwelled on the past, avoided people, avoided activities, procrastinated, and sought reassurance). Of note, down/depressed, frightened/afraid, and worthless/guilty were measured as couplets in a single item to reflect the language used in clinical assessment for anxiety or depression, and to prevent patients from being overly exclusive in endorsing them. The within- and between-person reliabilities for the scales were computed using procedures outlined by Cranford et al. (2006). For negative emotions, the within- and between-person reliabilities were 0.81 and 0.77, respectively; for positive emotions, they were 0.82 and 0.58, respectively.

Procedure

Clinical Interview

Following a brief telephone screening, eligible participants were invited to an in-person appointment for a structured clinical interview. The HRSD (along with other measures reported in Fisher et al., 2019) was administered by clinical psychology graduate students supervised by a doctoral-level clinical psychologist.2 At this appointment, participants also completed a battery of self-report measures, including the DASS.

EMA Surveys

After enrolling in the study, participants’ mobile phone numbers were entered into a secure web-based survey system which prompted participants to answer survey questions four times per day during pre-reported waking hours. During these hours, they received text messages (containing a hyperlink to a web-based survey) approximately every 4 h, with the exact time being randomized within a 30-min window. Each survey expired once a subsequent survey was sent. Participants were instructed to complete surveys for a minimum of 30 days (the total number of days ranged from 29 to 42; M = 34.25).

Personalized Treatment

Following the 30-day EMA period, participants started modular cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy for mood and anxiety disorders which was personalized via the selection of relevant modules from the unified protocol (Barlow et al., 2011) based on the EMA data (Fernandez et al., 2017; Fisher et al., 2019). The average number of sessions delivered in the study was 10.38, ranging from 4 to 14 (mode = 9). Within days of completing treatment, participants underwent an in-person follow-up assessment to evaluate change in diagnosis and symptoms severity. At this assessment, trained graduate students and postdoctoral therapists administered a structured clinical interview, and participants completed various self-report instruments, including the DASS.

Data Preparation

Data were processed and analyzed using R (version 4.0.3; R Core Team, 2020). Complete R syntax for the analyses described in this paper is available in the OSM (see footnote 1). Initially, composite positive and negative emotion scores were calculated for each time point of each participant by averaging across positive and negative emotion items. Next, means and standard deviations of these positive and negative emotions composites were calculated for each participant’s time series.

Subsequently, negative and positive ED indices were calculated for each participant using the average consistency3 intra-class correlation (ICC; Shrout and Fleiss, 1979), which is a standard procedure (e.g., Erbas et al., 2018). Resulting ICCs were transformed using a Fisher Z-transformation. To ease interpretation, we subtracted the transformed ICCs from 1.00 so that higher values will represent greater differentiation. No negative ICC values were obtained.

Data Analysis

To estimate the extent to which ED predicts treatment response, we conducted a series of hierarchical multiple regression models. In the final block, the pre- to post-treatment changes in DASS depression-stress and anxiety scales and the HRSD scores were regressed on (a) ED, (b) affect mean, (c) affect SD (representing emotional variability), and (d) the corresponding pre-treatment outcome measure scores. The means and SDs of momentary affect were included following Dejonckheere et al.’s (2019) recommendations to account for their shared variance with the ED indices.4 They were added iteratively to the models after the ED score (and the relevant pre-treatment outcome index) was the only predictor in the first block. All variables were standardized to ease the interpretation of the results. Separate models were estimated for each outcome measure (DASS depression-stress, DASS anxiety, and the HRSD) and for each affective valence (negative and positive emotions). Hence, six models were estimated in total.

To aid the interpretation of significant results vis-à-vis the small sample size, we estimated Bayesian regression models (against an intercept-only null hypothesis) using the BayesFactor package (Morey et al., 2018) parallel to the last steps in the models. For the effects of interest, we present Bayesian credible intervals based on the posterior distribution. Bayesian credible intervals refer directly to the probability of the parameter value to be within the intervals (unlike confidence intervals which refer to the probability of the interval itself to include the true value).

Results

The total number of observations per participant ranged from 90 to 151 (M = 113.19, SD = 11.83). The percentage of missing data ranged from 0 to 31.8% (M = 12.4%, SD = 8.5%). The intercorrelations among the ED indices, affect means, SDs, and outcome variables, as well as these variables’ means and SDs, are presented in Table 1. Among the affect indices, the only significant correlations were between negative ED and negative emotional variability (EV; r = −0.50) and between negative and positive EV (r = 0.69). Of the correlations between the affect indices and pre-treatment symptoms, the correlations between negative affect mean and DASS depression-stress (r = 0.48; notably, correlations with the DASS anxiety and the HRSD were 0.27 and 0.34, respectively) and between negative EV and DASS anxiety (r = 0.37)5 reached significance.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations.

Of note, the dependent variable in all models was a change score calculated by subtracting pre-treatment symptoms scores from the post-treatment symptoms scores. Hence, a more positive regression coefficient indicates that the predictor is associated with lower symptom reduction. A more negative coefficient indicates that the predictor is associated with greater symptom reduction.

Negative ED and Treatment Outcome

Predicting DASS Scores

The results of the hierarchical regression models predicting changes in DASS depression-stress and anxiety symptoms by negative affect indices are presented in Table 2’s left and right panels, respectively.6,7,8 For the depression-stress outcome, when negative ED was the only dynamic index in the model, it did not significantly predict change scores. After introducing the negative EV index, negative ED became a significant predictor with greater differentiation associated with greater symptom reduction. The negative EV index also predicted greater symptom reduction. Lastly, across blocks, higher levels of pre-treatment symptoms predicted greater change. These associations (except the ones with pre-treatment symptoms) did not reach significance in the model predicting anxiety symptoms change.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hierarchical linear regressions predicting DASS pre- to post-change by negative emotion indices.

To further explore the apparent suppression effect, we examined the associations between negative EV and symptoms change scores while adjusting for pre-treatment symptoms scores. These too were not significant, indicating a cooperative (or mutual) suppression (Cohen and Cohen, 1975). Statistically, each of the variables suppressed irrelevant (i.e., residual) variance (in predicting treatment outcomes) in each other. To estimate the size and robustness of the suppression effects, we followed recommendations by Shrout and Bolger (2002), who suggested considering suppression situations as a type of intervening variables models (e.g., mediation; see also Paulhus et al., 2004). Hence, we employed bootstrapping techniques (using the R package lavaan) to calculate the confidence intervals of the “indirect effect,” once with negative EV as the “mediator” and once with negative ED as the “mediator.” The bias-corrected bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals with 10,000 samples were above zero when EV [0.04, 0.53] and ED [0.03, 0.57] functioned as “mediators.”

Despite the small sample size, we chose to examine the non-independence of negative ED and negative EV by introducing their interaction term into the regression model. As shown in the lower panel of Table 2, the interaction term was below the threshold of statistical significance at alpha = 0.05 (p = 0.084) yet had medium effect sizes (ηp2 = 0.11). Moreover, adding the interaction term to the model accounted for an additional 4% of the variance. Hence, we further explored the simple slopes of the associations between negative ED, and symptoms change for different negative EV levels (one SD below and one SD above the mean of negative EV; see Figure 1—the y-axis uses the non-standardized difference scores to ease interpretation). The association between negative ED and the DASS depression-stress was negative and significant for low negative EV (coefficient = −0.56, SE = 0.17, t = −3.24, p < 0.001) and non-significant for high negative EV (coefficient = −0.08, SE = 0.23, t = −0.34, p = 0.74), indicating that for low negative EV, greater negative ED was associated with greater symptom reduction, whereas for high negative EV, it was not. Exploring the simple slopes of EV for different ED levels showed that the association between negative EV and changes in DASS depression-stress was negative and significant for low negative ED (coefficient = −0.58, SE = 0.19, t = −3.04, p < 0.001), and non-significant for high negative ED (coefficient = −0.10, SE = 0.20, t = −0.34, p = 0.63), indicating that for low negative ED, greater negative EV was associated with greater symptom reduction, whereas for high negative ED, it was not.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The associations between negative emotion differentiation and DASS depression-stress change scores for high (+1 SD) and low (−1 SD) levels of negative emotion variability. More negative change scores indicate grater symptom reduction.

Given the limited statistical power in the present study, the interaction results should be interpreted with caution. Notwithstanding, in the Bayesian regression model, the empirical means of negative ED and its interaction term with negative EV were −0.28 and 0.21, respectively, only slightly lower than their estimates in the original model. The respective 95% credible intervals were [−0.58, 0.02] and [−0.05, 0.47], respectively. Notably, in none of the models, negative emotion means predicted symptomatic change.

Predicting HRSD Scores

The results of the hierarchical regression models predicting changes in the HRSD by negative affect indices are presented in Table 3. Neither negative ED nor negative EV predicted HRSD change scores. Still, following the observed dependency between the predictors, we again added their interaction to the HRSD model. In this model, the interaction term reached statistical significance and accounted for an additional 10% of the variance. We explored the simple slopes (see Figure 2) and found that the effect of negative ED was negative and significant for low negative EV (coefficient = −0.48, SE = 0.18, t = −2.64, p = 0.01), and non-significant for high negative EV (coefficient = 0.38, SE = 0.28, t = 1.36, p = 0.19), indicating that for low negative EV, negative ED was associated with symptom reduction, whereas for high negative EV, it was not. In the Bayesian regression model, the empirical mean of the interaction term between ED and EV was 0.36, and the 95% credible intervals were [0.03, 0.69]. Notably, in none of the models, negative emotion means predicted symptomatic change.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Hierarchical linear regressions predicting Hamilton Depression Rating Scale pre- to post-change by negative emotion indices.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The associations between negative emotion differentiation and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale change scores for high (+1 SD) and low (−1 SD) levels of negative emotion variability. More negative change scores indicate grater symptom reduction.

Positive ED and Treatment Outcome

Predicting DASS Scores

The results of the hierarchical regression models predicting changes in DASS depression-stress and anxiety symptoms by positive affect indices are presented in Table 4’s left and right panels, respectively. Positive ED did not significantly predict change scores in either DASS depression-stress or anxiety. Positive emotion mean did predict greater changes in anxiety symptoms.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Hierarchical linear regressions predicting DASS pre- to post-change by positive emotion indices.

Predicting HRSD Scores

The results of the hierarchical regression models predicting changes in the HRSD by positive affect indices are presented in Table 5. No significant effects emerged.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Hierarchical linear regressions predicting Hamilton Depression Rating Scale pre- to post-change by positive emotion indices.

Discussion

The interest in pre-treatment dynamic assessment based on intensive repeated measurements taken in individuals’ daily life is rapidly growing (Fisher, 2015; Piccirillo and Rodebaugh, 2019; Wright and Zimmermann, 2019; Trull and Ebner-Priemer, 2020), demonstrating the immense potential it holds for clinical science and practice. Such assessment can be used to generate idiographic treatment plans (e.g., Fisher et al., 2019; Wright and Woods, 2020), but can also be employed with a more modest yet important aim of identifying predictors of treatment response. Dynamic assessment is particularly suitable to measure affective processes that unfold in time and reflect individuals’ capacity to process and regulate their emotions. The present work sought to explore one such capacity—individuals’ ability or tendency to differentiate between their emotions.

We estimated ED using an EMA paradigm of 1 month prior to cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy and examined its associations with self-report and clinician-administered outcome measures. Negative ED was found to be negatively associated with negative EV (a risk for multicollinearity problems in the ensuing regression models was largely allayed by low VIF values). Zero-order correlations between negative or positive ED and pre-treatment symptoms did not reach statistical significance. These non-significant correlations may reflect no true correlation in our purely clinical sample, but also the small sample size.

Negative ED was not independently associated with changes in any of the measures. Still, after introducing negative EV into the prediction models, the associations between negative ED and changes in self-reported depression and stress symptoms became significant. Negative EV itself was also not independently associated with change scores, but when concurrently estimated alongside negative ED, it was associated with the depression and stress self-reported change score.

Negative ED and negative EV acted as mutual suppressors increasing each other’s predictive validity once included in the same model (e.g., Paulhus et al., 2004). The shared variance between the two, which underlies the suppression effect, can stem from their common origin in the variance of patients’ momentary affect reports.9 Their shared variance may represent their ties with changes in the external contexts patients were exposed to during the EMA period. Greater contextual variability may elicit greater EV and also create the appearance of lower ED (because such changes make it easier for emotions to change together, that is, to be less differentiated). Including ED and EV in the same model allows for examining their effects while taking into account such hypothesized between-patient differences in contextual variability so that purer operationalizations of the processes of interest can be tested. Mutual suppression effects are statistically counter-intuitive yet make much theoretical sense. For example, guilt and shame, which are similar in being “self-conscious,” yet distinct in their objects (the former involves the global self, and the latter involves a specific behavior), were found to act as mutual suppressors in predicting aggression (Paulhus et al., 2004). Excluding self-conscious aggression-irrelevant variance revealed shame and guilt’s “true” predictive power. Future work employing larger samples within different contexts should explore the replicability and generalizability of our suppression finding.

The ED literature contains ample evidence for ED’s independent (i.e., not suppressed) associations with various wellbeing indicators (Seah and Coifman, 2021) and for its protective role in the face of daily stressors or maladaptive behaviors (e.g., Starr et al., 2017; Seah et al., 2020; Nook et al., 2021). ED’s role as a predictor of change processes is yet to be established. However, preliminary findings point to more complex relationships involving interactions between romantic partners’ ED (Lazarus et al., 2021a), between negative and positive ED (Liu et al., 2020), and between ED and personality traits (Oh and Tong, 2020). It seems that associations between ED and changes in outcome measures may be more specific and contingent on other factors.

To further explore the meaning of the non-independence between negative ED and negative EV in predicting self-reported symptoms changes, we added their interaction term to the prediction models. Given the small sample size, the interpretation of these interactions should be made cautiously. The interaction term did not reach statistical significance in predicting the self-report change scores but accounted for a considerable amount of their variance. Hence, we examined negative ED’s effects under different levels of negative EV and found that it was associated with changes in self-reported depression and stress symptoms for low, but not high, level of negative EV. Moreover, in the model predicting clinician-rated change in depression symptoms, the interaction term reached significance, with negative ED being associated with symptoms change only for low levels of negative EV.

Taken together, these findings indicate that pre-treatment negative ED may predict more favorable treatment response for those patients whose momentary experiences of negative emotions are less variable across time. We hypothesize that those patients whose negative emotions are less variable across time have a greater need to differentiate between these emotions due to the persistent or entrenched nature of their negative emotional experiences. Conversely, patients whose negative emotions are more variable across time may be able to benefit from psychotherapy even when they are less capable of differentiating between them. For these patients, their affect and symptomatology may be relatively malleable or plastic. Supporting this hypothesis, Shalom et al. (2020) found that variability in social anxiety symptoms (that include some affective items) before psychotherapy is predictive of sudden gains during the treatment.

Positive ED was not associated with any of the treatment response measures in the current study. Importantly, this finding should not be automatically generalized to other psychopathological conditions or other types of treatments. Positive ED was found to be associated with adaptive outcomes in contexts where positive emotions are prevalent or important (e.g., the transition to parenthood; Lazarus et al., 2021a). From a functional perspective, differentiated experience of positive emotions can aid in eliciting specific and adapted motivational, cognitive, physiological, and behavioral responses to environmental opportunities (Shiota et al., 2014; Beall and Tracy, 2017). In the context of interventions targeting positive affect and reward sensitivity (e.g., Craske et al., 2019), positive ED may prove beneficial.

Of note, the means of patients’ negative and positive emotions throughout the EMA period were not associated with symptomatic change (except for the association between positive emotion mean and changes in anxiety symptoms). These null effects partially echo previous work exploring daily affect and psychotherapy response. Specifically, Husen et al. (2016) found that mean positive and negative daily affect did not predict early response in cognitive-behavioral therapy. In Forbes et al. (2012), mean daily negative (but not positive) affect was tied to a slower rate of symptom reduction during depression and anxiety treatment for children and adolescents. In both studies, greater positive to negative emotions ratio predicted better treatment response (we did not observe a similar pattern in our data). It is notable that the significant predictors of symptomatic change (ED, EV, and positive to negative affect ratio indices) all involve within-person (co)variation, unlike the means, which represent a summary of absolute values. Absolute values may be more liable to various response biases that restrict their efficiency in predicting change scores. While the diverse research contexts and limited sample sizes (N = 39 in Husen et al., 2016; N = 66 in Forbes et al., 2012) make it difficult to draw firm conclusions, this emergent pattern may strengthen the case for the predictive validity of dynamic indices vs. mean levels.

Identifying patients who fail to sufficiently differentiate between their negative emotions in daily life can guide therapists’ efforts at the first treatment stages. Therapists can employ various techniques and tools developed in the context of leading clinical approaches, including emotion-focused therapy (e.g., Pascual-Leone and Greenberg, 2007) and cognitive-behavioral therapy (e.g., Barlow et al., 2011) to help their patients attain a more differentiated emotional experience. Other capacities, such as mindfulness skills (Van der Gucht et al., 2019), or activities, such as self-monitoring (e.g., Widdershoven et al., 2019), have been shown to improve ED. Patients can then use this newly acquired ability to achieve other therapeutic goals.

Broader Considerations

This study examined a specific EMA-derived patient factor predictive of treatment response. Current efforts to identify patient factors often adopt data-driven machine learning algorithms that examine large numbers of possible predictors, with the potential to estimate nonlinear associations and higher-order interactions (e.g., Zilcha-Mano, 2019; Webb et al., 2020). Despite the advantages this approach may hold, it suffers from several limitations. First, generalizable findings require very large sample sizes (Archer et al., 2021) often unavailable in psychotherapy context. Second, the resultant models are often a black box with limited interpretability. Third, in the context of psychotherapy outcome prediction, this approach usually relies on self-reports. Arguably, the quality of any statistical model is limited by the quality of the data it includes, and single-time self-reports are inherently limited in their ability to capture dynamic processes representative of prospective patients’ abilities. Due to these limitations, we believe that a theory-driven EMA-based search for specific treatment outcome predictors is necessary and valuable.

A significant advantage of dynamic assessment is that its reliance on associations between repeatedly measured self-report variables helps alleviate the risk of patients being swayed by factors, such as social desirability or experimenter demands typical of single-time self-report assessment (Sened et al., 2018). This risk may be particularly relevant in the context of pre-therapy assessment, where prospective patients may either over (Merckelbach et al., 2019) or under (Warner et al., 2011) report their psychological difficulties and symptoms. Using dynamic within individual patterns as predictors allows researchers to go beyond respondents’ direct awareness and the mean levels of their reports, thus increasing these predictors’ validity.

The discovered interactive effect between ED and emotional variability may suggest that greater attention to interactions between affect dynamics in their relations with other constructs is in place. After all, it is unlikely that these relations follow simple linear regularities, but rather more complex patterns (e.g., Wichers et al., 2015). It is possible that interactions between different dynamic indices will function better than single indices in representing robust interindividual differences. Notably, examining such interactive effects will require increased sample sizes.

In this study, the dynamic indices were derived from surveys collected four times a day, approximately 4 h apart. This data collection scheme was chosen to reduce patients’ burden and provide a representative sample of participants waking hours, but the relatively long measurement intervals run the risk of missing the more rapid affective processes (e.g., Verduyn et al., 2009). An alternative, contextualized approach to dynamic assessment may aim to capture affect dynamics when and where they matter the most, for example, in the vicinity of a stressful event (Dejonckheere et al., 2020; Lapate and Heller, 2020). Moreover, assessment of affect dynamics can be relevant and informative also after psychotherapy has started using either EMA between sessions (e.g., Frumkin et al., 2020) or reports regarding the sessions themselves (Lazarus et al., 2019; Galili-Weinstock et al., 2020).

Lastly, for dynamic assessment of affective processes to reach its full potential, it must involve thorough consideration of the temporal dynamics of the target processes (e.g., Hamaker and Wichers, 2017; Lazarus et al., 2021b). Specifically, a time scale (e.g., Adolf et al., 2021) appropriate for capturing affective changes as they unfold in patients’ daily life should be identified based on prior research (e.g., Verduyn et al., 2009) or theoretical grounds, and dictate the measurement scheme. Additionally, trends (e.g., linear and quadratic; Jebb et al., 2015) and cycles (e.g., diurnal and weekly; van de Maat et al., 2020) should be modeled and interpreted on a case-by-case basis (Fisher and Newman, 2016).

Limitations

Several limitations of the present study should be acknowledged. First, the available sample size of treatment completers provided low statistical power. Such low power may have prevented us from detecting some effects that would have emerged with a larger sample. This sample size should also suggest caution when interpreting the effects that did emerge, as they may not be generalizable to other samples. Clearly, replications with larger samples are necessary to establish the reported effects’ validity. Of note, the study’s procedure is highly demanding (included both EMA and psychotherapy) and makes larger samples hard to obtain. Moreover, the study did have a large number of within-individual measurements across a prolonged period, increasing the ED indices’ reliability.

Second, though ED was measured prior to treatment, claims regarding its causal role in the treatment should be taken with caution. While we cannot rule out the effects of many “third variables,” the inclusion of pre-treatment symptoms scores in all models narrows this concern somewhat. Future work measuring ED throughout the treatment and at its end can provide further credibility for causal inferences.

Third, the items used to estimate patients’ ED suffered from two limitations stemming from the original focus and the purpose of the data collection. The PED measure was based on only four positive emotions and included one unspecific item (i.e., “positive”). This narrow measurement was meant to reduce participants burden but might have crippled our positive ED index. Given the growing interest in the role of distinct positive emotions (e.g., Weidman and Tracy, 2020), future dynamic assessment work should operationalize positive ED using a larger number of items. The NED measure was partially based on items with conjoint terms. Two of these items (i.e., down/depressed and frightened/afraid) were used to avoid patients being overly exclusive in endorsing them; the third (worthless/guilty) was used as an adaptation of one of the key DSM depression symptoms and involves clearly two distinct emotions. These issues might have added noise to our NED measurement. Specifically, endorsement of the same value for these items on different occasions may reflect different experiences for patients who could differentiate between the conjoint terms. Consequently, the ED scores of these patients might have been underestimated. The usage of such items is particularly problematic when studying ED because how individuals interpret them is a derivative of ED itself and thus runs the risk of leaving important between-individual variability in ED unaccounted for. Furthermore, the negative affect measurement included a limited number of items, and future work is necessary to assess whether findings generalize to differentiation among other emotions.

Conclusion

The present work took a preliminary step in demonstrating the utility of dynamic assessment to identify affect-processing patient factors predictive of treatment outcome. We found that negative ED predicted better treatment response when emotional variability was taken into account. Our findings suggest that negative ED may play an important role in the success of psychotherapeutic interventions.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material; further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Committee for Protection of Human Subjects (CPHS), University of California, Berkeley. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

GL developed the idea for the study and conducted the data analyses and interpretation under the guidance of AF. AF performed the data collection. GL drafted the paper under the guidance of AF who provided critical revisions and comments. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

GL is supported by a Marie Skłodowska-Curie individual fellowship (895828) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program. The Berkeley Research Impact Initiative (BRII) provides funding for the publication fees. Publication made possible in part by support from the Berkeley Research Impact Initiative (BRII) sponsored by the UC Berkeley Library.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^https://osf.io/vqsdb

2. ^Inter-rater reliability was calculated only for the ADIS-5 diagnoses.

3. ^We opted to use the consistency index that ignores reported items’ means, as we were concerned the latter may reflect response tendencies and not true differentiation. The correlations between the consistency and absolute agreement indices were 0.95 and 0.96 for the negative and positive indices, respectively.

4. ^Due to the small sample size we opted not to include additional affect dynamics indices.

5. ^We avoided noting the simple correlations with the difference scores because the associations between the pre-treatment symptoms and (a) post-treatment symptoms (r = 0.42, 0.43, and 0.47 for DASS depression-stress, DASS anxiety, and the HRSD, respectively) were moderate and significant, and (b) the affect indices were non-zero. These associations make difference-scores correlations hard to interpret (Allison, 1990).

6. ^We reran all models while adjusting for gender, age, and number of EMA surveys completed. Results remained essentially unchanged.

7. ^To allay the concern of multicollinearity, we examined the variance inflation factor (VIF) of the predictors in all models. All the values were below 1.86, indicating that the models did not suffer from a multicollinearity problem.

8. ^At the request of a reviewer, we examined additional models predicting each subscale separately and a model predicting the DASS total score. The results of these models are presented in Tables S2 and S3 in the OSM. As can be seen in Table S2 (NA indices), whereas no significant effects emerged for the separate DASS subscales, effects parallel to the combined DASS depression-stress measure emerged for the DASS total score. As can be seen in Table S3 (PA indices), no significant effects emerged for any of the outcome variables.

9. ^Whereas EV represents the variance in negative affect reports attributable to differences between measurements, ED represents the variance in affect reports not attributable to differences between measurements and between specific items’ means (that is, their inconsistency).

References

Aafjes-van Doorn, K., and Barber, J. P. (2017). Systematic review of in-session affect experience in cognitive behavioral therapy for depression. Cognit. Ther. Res. 41, 807–828. doi: 10.1007/s10608-017-9865-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adolf, J. K., Loossens, T., Tuerlinckx, F., and Ceulemans, E. (2021). Optimal sampling rates for reliable continuous-time first-order autoregressive and vector autoregressive modeling. Psychol. Methods. doi: 10.1037/met0000398 [Epub ahead of print]

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, P. D. (1990). Change scores as dependent variables in regression analysis. Sociol. Methodol. 20, 93–114. doi: 10.2307/271083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, L., Snell, K. I., Ensor, J., Hudda, M. T., Collins, G. S., and Riley, R. D. (2021). Minimum sample size for external validation of a clinical prediction model with a continuous outcome. Stat. Med. 40, 133–146. doi: 10.1002/sim.8766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, S. A., and Imel, Z. E. (2013). “Therapist effects: findings and methods,” in Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. 6th Edn. ed. M. J. Lambert (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 259–297.

Google Scholar

Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Boisseau, C. L., Allen, L. B., et al. (2011). Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emotional Disorders: Therapist Guide. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., and Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognit. Emot. 15, 713–724. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beall, A. T., and Tracy, J. L. (2017). Emotivational psychology: how distinct emotions facilitate fundamental motives. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 11:e12303. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohart, A. C., and Wade, A. G. (2013). “The client in psychotherapy,” in Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. 6th Edn. ed. M. J. Lambert (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 219–257.

Google Scholar

Bos, E. H., de Jonge, P., and Cox, R. F. (2019). Affective variability in depression: revisiting the inertia–instability paradox. Br. J. Psychol. 110, 814–827. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosley, H. G., Soyster, P. D., and Fisher, A. J. (2019). Affect dynamics as predictors of symptom severity and treatment response in mood and anxiety disorders: evidence for specificity. J. Pers. Oriented Res. 5, 101–113. doi: 10.17505/jpor.2019.09

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boswell, J. F., Constantino, M. J., and Anderson, L. M. (2016). “Potential obstacles to treatment success in adults: client characteristics,” in Oxford Library of Psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Treatment Processes and Outcomes in Psychology: A Multidisciplinary, Biopsychosocial Approach. ed. S. Maltzman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 183–205.

Google Scholar

Brown, T. A., and Barlow, D. H. (2014). Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-5: Clinician Manual. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Campbell-Sills, L., and Brown, T. A. (2010). “Generalized anxiety disorder,” in Handbook of Assessment and Treatment Planning for Psychological Disorders. eds. M. M. Antony and D. H. Barlow (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 224–266.

Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., and Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 316–336. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J., and Cohen, P. (1975). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Constantino, M. J., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., Ametrano, R. M., and Smith, J. Z. (2011). “Expectations,” in Psychotherapy Relationships That Work: Evidence Based Responsiveness. 2nd Edn. ed. J. C. Norcross (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 354–376.

Google Scholar

Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., and Bolger, N. (2006). A procedure for evaluating sensitivity to within-person change: can mood measures in diary studies detect change reliably? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 917–929. doi: 10.1177/0146167206287721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craske, M. G., Meuret, A. E., Ritz, T., Treanor, M., Dour, H., and Rosenfield, D. (2019). Positive affect treatment for depression and anxiety: A randomized clinical trial for a core feature of anhedonia. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 87, 457–471. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Smit, F., and Andersson, G. (2009). Is psychotherapy for depression equally effective in younger and older adults? A meta-regression analysis. Int. Psychogeriatr. 21, 16–24. doi: 10.1017/S1041610208008089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dejonckheere, E., Mestdagh, M., Houben, M., Rutten, I., Sels, L., Kuppens, P., et al. (2019). Complex affect dynamics add limited information to the prediction of psychological well-being. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 478–491. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0555-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dejonckheere, E., Mestdagh, M., Kuppens, P., and Tuerlinckx, F. (2020). Reply to: Context matters for affective chronometry. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 690–693. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0861-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demiralp, E., Thompson, R. J., Mata, J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Barrett, L. F., et al. (2012). Feeling blue or turquoise? Emotional differentiation in major depressive disorder. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1410–1416. doi: 10.1177/0956797612444903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Chapman, A. L., Weiss, N. H., and Rosenthal, M. Z. (2014). A preliminary examination of the role of emotion differentiation in the relationship between borderline personality and urges for maladaptive behaviors. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 36, 616–625. doi: 10.1007/s10862-014-9423-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dryman, M. T., and Heimberg, R. G. (2018). Emotion regulation in social anxiety and depression: a systematic review of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 65, 17–42. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., Kalokerinos, E. K., Houben, M., Koval, P., Pe, M. L., et al. (2018). Why I don’t always know what I’m feeling: the role of stress in within-person fluctuations in emotion differentiation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 115, 179–191. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez, K. C., Fisher, A. J., and Chi, C. (2017). Development and initial implementation of the Dynamic Assessment Treatment Algorithm (DATA). PLoS One 12:e0178806. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, N., Saxon, D., Stiles, W. B., and Barkham, M. (2019). Therapist and clinic effects in psychotherapy: A three-level model of outcome variability. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 87, 345–356. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, A. J. (2015). Toward a dynamic model of psychological assessment: implications for personalized care. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 83, 825–836. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, H., Atzil-Slonim, D., Bar-Kalifa, E., Rafaeli, E., and Peri, T. (2016). Emotional experience and alliance contribute to therapeutic change in psychodynamic therapy. Psychotherapy 53, 105–116. doi: 10.1037/pst0000041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, A. J., Bosley, H. G., Fernandez, K. C., Reeves, J. W., Soyster, P. D., Diamond, A. E., et al. (2019). Open trial of a personalized modular treatment for mood and anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 116, 69–79. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, A. J., and Boswell, J. F. (2016). Enhancing the personalization of psychotherapy with dynamic assessment and modeling. Assessment 23, 496–506. doi: 10.1177/1073191116638735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, A. J., and Newman, M. G. (2016). Reductions in the diurnal rigidity of anxiety predict treatment outcome in cognitive behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 79, 46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes, E. E., Stepp, S. D., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N. D., Whalen, D., Axelson, D. A., et al. (2012). Real-world affect and social context as predictors of treatment response in child and adolescent depression and anxiety: an ecological momentary assessment study. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 22, 37–47. doi: 10.1089/cap.2011.0085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frumkin, M. R., Piccirillo, M. L., Beck, E. D., Grossman, J. T., and Rodebaugh, T. L. (2020). Feasibility and utility of idiographic models in the clinic: a pilot study. Psychother. Res. 31, 520–534. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2020.1805133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galili-Weinstock, L., Lazarus, G., Atzil-Slonim, D., Bar-Kalifa, E., Rafaeli, E., and Peri, T. (2020). Self-compassion among psychotherapy clients is in the details of negative, not positive, emotions. J. Posit. Psychol. 15, 478–487. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1627396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, L. S. (2012). Emotions, the great captains of our lives: their role in the process of change in psychotherapy. Am. Psychol. 67, 697–707. doi: 10.1037/a0029858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, L. S., and Safran, J. D. (1989). Emotion in psychotherapy. Am. Psychol. 44, 19–29. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.1.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamaker, E. L., and Wichers, M. (2017). No time like the present: discovering the hidden dynamics in intensive longitudinal data. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 10–15. doi: 10.1177/0963721416666518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, M., and Newman, M. G. (2006). “Generalized anxiety disorder,” in Comprehensive Handbook of Personality and Psychopathology: Vol. II. Adult Psychopathology. series eds. M. Hersen and C. Thomas, vol. ed. F. Andrasik (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons), 101–120.

Google Scholar

Hosogoshi, H., Takebayashi, Y., Ito, M., Fujisato, H., Kato, N., Nakajima, S., et al. (2020). Expressive suppression of emotion is a moderator of anxiety in a unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of anxiety and depressive disorders: a secondary analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 277, 1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Husen, K., Rafaeli, E., Rubel, J. A., Bar-Kalifa, E., and Lutz, W. (2016). Daily affect dynamics predict early response in CBT: Feasibility and predictive validity of EMA for outpatient psychotherapy. J. Affect. Disord. 206, 305–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jebb, A. T., Tay, L., Wang, W., and Huang, Q. (2015). Time series analysis for psychological research: examining and forecasting change. Front. Psychol. 6:727. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalokerinos, E. K., Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., and Kuppens, P. (2019). Differentiate to regulate: low negative emotion differentiation is associated with ineffective use but not selection of emotion-regulation strategies. Psychol. Sci. 30, 863–879. doi: 10.1177/0956797619838763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F., and McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation: transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 10–16. doi: 10.1177/0963721414550708

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., and Farmer, A. S. (2014). Differentiating emotions across contexts: comparing adults with and without social anxiety disorder using random, social interaction, and daily experience sampling. Emotion 14, 629–638. doi: 10.1037/a0035796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuppens, P., Oravecz, Z., and Tuerlinckx, F. (2010). Feelings change: accounting for individual differences in the temporal dynamics of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 1042–1060. doi: 10.1037/a0020962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, M. J. (2013). “The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy,” in Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. 6th Edn. ed. M. J. Lambert (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 169–218.

Google Scholar

Lapate, R. C., and Heller, A. S. (2020). Context matters for affective chronometry. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 688–689. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0860-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, G., Atzil-Slonim, D., Bar-Kalifa, E., Hasson-Ohayon, I., and Rafaeli, E. (2019). Clients’ emotional instability and therapists’ inferential flexibility predict therapists’ session-by-session empathic accuracy. J. Couns. Psychol. 66, 56–69. doi: 10.1037/cou0000310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, G., Pshedetzky-Shohat, R., Zahavi, T., Gleason, M. J., and Rafaeli, E. (2021a). Emotion differentiation during the transition to parenthood – concurrent and prospective positive effects. [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/7grf2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, G., Song, J., Crawford, C., and Fisher, A. J. (2021b). “A close look at the role of time in affect dynamics research,” in Affective Dynamics. eds. C. Waugh and P. Kuppens (New York, NY: Springer).

Google Scholar

Liu, D. Y., Gilbert, K. E., and Thompson, R. J. (2020). Emotion differentiation moderates the effects of rumination on depression: a longitudinal study. Emotion 20, 1234–1243. doi: 10.1037/emo0000627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luborsky, L., Auerbach, A. H., Chandler, M., Cohen, J., and Bachrach, H. M. (1971). Factors influencing the outcome of psychotherapy: a review of quantitative research. Psychol. Bull. 75, 145–185. doi: 10.1037/h0030480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, W., Schwartz, B., Hofmann, S. G., Fisher, A. J., Husen, K., and Rubel, J. A. (2018). Using network analysis for the prediction of treatment dropout in patients with mood and anxiety disorders: a methodological proof-of-concept study. Sci. Rep. 8:7819. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25953-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, D. K., O’Connell, D., Norris, A. L., and Sawaqdeh, A. (2014). Is the dodo bird endangered in the 21st century? A meta-analysis of treatment comparison studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 34, 519–530. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., and Fresco, D. M. (2002). Applying an emotion regulation framework to integrative approaches to generalized anxiety disorder. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 9, 85–90. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.9.1.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merckelbach, H., Dandachi-FitzGerald, B., van Helvoort, D., Jelicic, M., and Otgaar, H. (2019). When patients overreport symptoms: more than just malingering. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 321–326. doi: 10.1177/0963721419837681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moras, K., Di Nardo, P. A., and Barlow, D. H. (1992). Distinguishing anxiety and depression: reexamination of the reconstructed Hamilton scales. Psychol. Assess. 4, 224–227. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.4.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morey, R. D., Rouder, J. N., and Jamil, T. (2018). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for common designs. R package version 0.9. 12-4.2. [Computer software]. Available at: https://cran.r-project.org/package=BayesFactor

Google Scholar

Newman, M. G., Crits-Christoph, P., Connolly Gibbons, M. B., and Erickson, T. M. (2006). “Participant factors in treating anxiety disorders,” in Principles of Therapeutic Change That Work. eds. L. G. Castonguay and L. E. Beutler (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 121–154.

Google Scholar

Nook, E. C., Flournoy, J. C., Rodman, A. M., Mair, P., and McLaughlin, K. A. (2021). High emotion differentiation buffers against internalizing symptoms following exposure to stressful life events in adolescence: an intensive longitudinal study. Clin. Psychol. Sci. [Preprint]. doi: 10.1177/2167702620979786

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norcross, J. C., and Lambert, M. J. (2011). “Evidence based therapy relationships,” in Psychotherapy Relationships That Work: Evidence Based Responsiveness. ed. J. C. Norcross (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 3–24.

Google Scholar

Oh, V. Y. S., and Tong, E. M. W. (2020). Negative emotion differentiation and long-term physical health—The moderating role of neuroticism. Health Psychol. 39, 127–136. doi: 10.1037/hea0000809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., and Greenberg, L. S. (2007). Emotional processing in experiential therapy: why “the only way out is through”. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 75, 875–887. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.6.875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., and Yeryomenko, N. (2017). The client “experiencing” scale as a predictor of treatment outcomes: a meta-analysis on psychotherapy process. Psychother. Res. 27, 653–665. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2016.1152409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., and Tracy, J. L. (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivar. Behav. Res. 39, 303–328. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and Seligman, M. E. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: theory and evidence. Psychol. Rev. 91, 347–374. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccirillo, M. L., and Rodebaugh, T. L. (2019). Foundations of idiographic methods in psychology and applications for psychotherapy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 71, 90–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinna, F., Manchia, M., Paribello, P., and Carpiniello, B. (2020). The impact of alexithymia on treatment response in psychiatric disorders: a systematic review. Front. Psych. 11:311. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pos, A. E., Greenberg, L. S., and Warwar, S. H. (2009). Testing a model of change in the experiential treatment for depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 77, 1055–1066. doi: 10.1037/a0017059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quilty, L. C., Taylor, G. J., McBride, C., and Bagby, R. M. (2017). Relationships among alexithymia, therapeutic alliance, and psychotherapy outcome in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 254, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.04.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing Available at: http://www.R-project.org

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rottenberg, J. (2017). Emotions in depression: what do we really know? Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 13, 241–263. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubel, J. A., Fisher, A. J., Husen, K., and Lutz, W. (2018). Translating person-specific network models into personalized treatments: development and demonstration of the Dynamic Assessment Treatment Algorithm for Individual Networks (DATA-IN). Psychother. Psychosom. 87, 249–251. doi: 10.1159/000487769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, A., Boecker, M., Pawelzik, M., Gauggel, S., and Forkmann, T. (2017). Emotion suppression, not reappraisal, predicts psychotherapy outcome. Psychother. Res. 27, 143–153. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2015.1080875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, N. (2012). “Feelings-as-information theory,” in Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology. eds. P. A. M. Lange Van, A. W. Kruglanski, and E. T. Higgins (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 289–308.

Google Scholar

Seah, T. S., Aurora, P., and Coifman, K. G. (2020). Emotion differentiation as a protective factor against the behavioral consequences of rumination: a conceptual replication and extension in the context of social anxiety. Behav. Ther. 51, 135–148. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2019.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seah, T. H. S., and Coifman, K. G. (2021). Emotion differentiation and behavioral dysregulation in clinical and non-clinical samples: a meta-analysis. Emotion. [Preprint]. doi: 10.1037/emo0000968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selby, E. A., Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., Engel, S. G., Panza, E., Mitchell, J. E., et al. (2014). Nothing tastes as good as thin feels. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 514–531. doi: 10.1177/2167702613512794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E., Abramson, L. Y., Semmel, A., and von Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 88, 242–247. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.88.3.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sened, H., Lazarus, G., Gleason, M. E., Rafaeli, E., and Fleeson, W. (2018). The use of intensive longitudinal methods in explanatory personality research. Eur. J. Personal. 32, 269–285. doi: 10.1002/per.2143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalom, J. G., Strauss, A. Y., Huppert, J. D., Andersson, G., Agassi, O. D., and Aderka, I. M. (2020). Predicting sudden gains before treatment begins: An examination of pre-treatment intraindividual variability in symptoms. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 88, 809–817. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Neufeld, S. L., Danvers, A. F., Osborne, E. A., Sng, O., and Yee, C. I. (2014). Positive emotion differentiation: a functional approach. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 8, 104–117. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol. Bull. 86, 420–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spek, V., Nykliček, I., Cuijpers, P., and Pop, V. (2008). Alexithymia and cognitive behaviour therapy outcome for subthreshold depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 118, 164–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01199.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr, L. R., Hershenberg, R., Li, Y. I., and Shaw, Z. A. (2017). When feelings lack precision: low positive and negative emotion differentiation and depressive symptoms in daily life. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 5, 613–631. doi: 10.1177/2167702617694657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steer, R. A., Beck, A. T., Riskind, J. H., and Brown, G. (1987). Relationships between the Beck depression inventory and the Hamilton psychiatric rating scale for depression in depressed outpatients. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 9, 327–339. doi: 10.1007/BF00964561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steer, R. A., McElroy, M. G., and Beck, A. T. (1983). Correlates of self-reported and clinically assessed depression in outpatient alcoholics. J. Clin. Psychol. 39, 144–149. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198301)39:1<144::AID-JCLP2270390128>3.0.CO;2-M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thoma, N. C., and McKay, D. (2015). Working with Emotion in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy: Techniques for Clinical Practice. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Torre, J. B., and Lieberman, M. D. (2018). Putting feelings into words: affect labeling as implicit emotion regulation. Emot. Rev. 10, 116–124. doi: 10.1177/1754073917742706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trull, T. J., and Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2020). Ambulatory assessment in psychopathology research: A review of recommended reporting guidelines and current practices. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 129, 56–63. doi: 10.1037/abn0000473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Maat, R., Lataster, J., and Verboon, P. (2020). Why and how to deal with diurnal cyclic patterns in ambulatory assessment of emotions. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 36, 471–481. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Gucht, K., Dejonckheere, E., Erbas, Y., Takano, K., Vandemoortele, M., Maex, E., et al. (2019). An experience sampling study examining the potential impact of a mindfulness-based intervention on emotion differentiation. Emotion 19, 123–131. doi: 10.1037/emo0000406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verduyn, P., Delvaux, E., Van Coillie, H., Tuerlinckx, F., and Van Mechelen, I. (2009). Predicting the duration of emotional experience: two experience sampling studies. Emotion 9, 83–91. doi: 10.1037/a0014610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wampold, B. E. (2010). “The research evidence for common factors models: a historically situated perspective,” in The Heart & Soul of Change: Delivering What Works. 2nd Edn. eds. B. L. Dunca, S. D. Miller, B. E. Wampold, and M. A. Hubble (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 49–82.

Google Scholar

Warner, C. H., Appenzeller, G. N., Grieger, T., Belenkiy, S., Breitbach, J., Parker, J., et al. (2011). Importance of anonymity to encourage honest reporting in mental health screening after combat deployment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 1065–1071. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, J. C., and Bedard, D. (2006). Clients’ emotional processing in psychotherapy: a comparison between cognitive-behavioral and process-experiential psychotherapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 74, 152–159. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.1.152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, J. C., McMullen, E. J., Prosser, M. C., and Bedard, D. L. (2011). An examination of the relationships among clients’ affect regulation, in-session emotional processing, the working alliance, and outcome. Psychother. Res. 21, 86–96. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2010.518637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, C. A., Cohen, Z. D., Beard, C., Forgeard, M., Peckham, A. D., and Björgvinsson, T. (2020). Personalized prognostic prediction of treatment outcome for depressed patients in a naturalistic psychiatric hospital setting: a comparison of machine learning approaches. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 88, 25–38. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weidman, A. C., and Tracy, J. L. (2020). A provisional taxonomy of subjectively experienced positive emotions. Affect. Sci. 1, 57–86. doi: 10.1007/s42761-020-00009-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, L. P., Wright, A. G., Pilkonis, P. A., Woods, W. C., Denissen, J. J., Kühnel, A., et al. (2020). Indicators of affect dynamics: structure, reliability, and personality correlates. Eur. J. Personal. 34, 1060–1072. doi: 10.1002/per.2277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichers, M., Wigman, J. T. W., and Myin-Germeys, I. (2015). Micro-level affect dynamics in psychopathology viewed from complex dynamical system theory. Emot. Rev. 7, 362–367. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widdershoven, R. L., Wichers, M., Kuppens, P., Hartmann, J. A., Menne-Lothmann, C., Simons, C. J., et al. (2019). Effect of self-monitoring through experience sampling on emotion differentiation in depression. J. Affect. Disord. 244, 71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.10.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willroth, E. C., Flett, J. A. M., and Mauss, I. B. (2020). Depressive symptoms and deficits in stress-reactive negative, positive, and within-emotion-category differentiation: A daily diary study. J. Pers. 88, 174–184. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, A. G., and Woods, W. C. (2020). Personalized models of psychopathology. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 16, 49–74. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-102419-125032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, A. G., and Zimmermann, J. (2019). Applied ambulatory assessment: Integrating idiographic and nomothetic principles of measurement. Psychol. Assess. 31, 1467–1480. doi: 10.1037/pas0000685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, L. F., Coifman, K. G., Rafaeli, E., Berenson, K. R., and Downey, G. (2013). Emotion differentiation as a protective factor against nonsuicidal self-injury in borderline personality disorder. Behav. Ther. 44, 529–540. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilcha-Mano, S. (2019). Major developments in methods addressing for whom psychotherapy may work and why. Psychother. Res. 29, 693–708. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1429691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion differentiation, dynamic assessment, psychotherapy outcome, patient factors, affect dynamics

Citation: Lazarus G and Fisher AJ (2021) Negative Emotion Differentiation Predicts Psychotherapy Outcome: Preliminary Findings. Front. Psychol. 12:689407. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.689407

Received: 01 April 2021; Accepted: 02 July 2021;
Published: 02 August 2021.

Edited by:

Yasemin Erbas, KU Leuven, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Erik Nook, Harvard University, United States
Meng Yu, Sun Yat-sen University, China
Stanley Seah, Kent State University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Lazarus and Fisher. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gal Lazarus, gal.lazarus@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.