Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2014

01-01-2014 | Original Article

Getting a tool gives wings: overestimation of tool-related benefits in a motor imagery task and a decision task

Auteurs: François Osiurak, Nicolas Morgado, Guillaume T. Vallet, Marion Drot, Richard Palluel-Germain

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Two experiments examine whether people overestimate the benefits provided by tool use in motor tasks. Participants had to move different quantities of objects by hand (two at a time) or with a tool (four at a time). The tool was not within reach so participants had to get it before moving the objects. In Experiment 1, the task was performed in a real and an imagined situation. In Experiment 2, participants had to decide for each quantity, whether they preferred moving the objects by hand or with the tool. Our findings indicated that people perceive tool actions as less costly in terms of movement time than they actually are (Experiment 1) and decide to use a tool even when it objectively provides less time-based benefits than using the hands (Experiment 2). Taken together, the data suggest that people overestimate the benefits provided by tool use.
Voetnoten
1
The difference between the Hand condition and the Tool condition in the Real situation is particularly important for Experiment 2, so we report here the differences (in s) between the Hand condition minus the Tool condition in the Real situation. Note that a positive difference reflects a gain in favor of the Hand condition, and a negative difference a gain in favor of the Tool condition: Quantity 4 (M = 10, SD = 1.39, range: 8:17), Quantity 8 (M = 6, SD = 3.52, range: 4:13), Quantity 12 (M = 0, SD = 3.38, range: −8:5), Quantity 16 (M = −6, SD = 4.44, range: −18:−1), Quantity 20 (M = −10, SD = 5.05, range: −19:−5), and Quantity 24 (M = −16; SD = 6.35, range: −29:−11). Importantly, these descriptive results indicate that the gain in favor of the Hand condition was present for all the participants for the quantities 4 and 8.
 
2
Imagery-related speed gain was submitted to an ANOVA with Path (A–C–A to get the tub vs. A–C–A to return the tub) and Quantity of rolls (4 vs. 8 vs. 12 vs. 16 vs. 20 vs. 24) as within-subject factors. The analysis revealed no significant main effects and no interaction between the factors (Fs < 1).
 
3
Another possibility to collect the choices could have been, for each trial, to present the participants with a quantity of rolls, to ask them to choose the condition in which they would move the rolls and to actually move the rolls. However, as explained in Experiment 1, moving the rolls in the Real situation required a substantial amount of time. So this procedure would have taken too much time, preventing the collection of several trials for each quantity. Moreover, we did not decide to ask the participants to actually move the rolls after the decision task for each quantity and in each condition (Tool and Hand). This procedure could have been interesting to compare the choices made by the participants with the time taken to actually move the rolls. Nevertheless, we thought that the participants might have modified their way to move the rolls to be consistent with the choices made during the decision task. We posited that the time taken by the participants of Experiment 2 to actually move the rolls for each quantity and in each condition (Tool and Hand) should be similar to that of the participants of Experiment 1. This assumption is based on the results of Experiment 1 indicating that the time-based benefit between the two conditions was globally constant among the participants of Experiment 1 (see Footnote 1).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use: Forms of engagement in human and animal use of tools. London: Talyor & Francis.CrossRef Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use: Forms of engagement in human and animal use of tools. London: Talyor & Francis.CrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, K. R. (1991). Tools, language and intelligence: Evolutionary implications. Man, 26, 255–264.CrossRef Gibson, K. R. (1991). Tools, language and intelligence: Evolutionary implications. Man, 26, 255–264.CrossRef
go back to reference Kunz, B. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2009). Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1458–1471. doi:10.1037/a0015786.PubMed Kunz, B. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2009). Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1458–1471. doi:10.​1037/​a0015786.PubMed
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540. doi:10.1037/a0019004.PubMedCrossRef Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540. doi:10.​1037/​a0019004.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–129. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08003543.PubMed Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–129. doi:10.​1017/​S0140525X0800354​3.PubMed
go back to reference van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1970). Tool-using in primates and other vertebrates. In D. Lehrman, R. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior (pp. 195–249). New York: Academic Press. van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1970). Tool-using in primates and other vertebrates. In D. Lehrman, R. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior (pp. 195–249). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156. doi:10.1037/a0021933.PubMed Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156. doi:10.​1037/​a0021933.PubMed
go back to reference Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492. doi:10.1037/a0010781.PubMedCentralPubMed Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492. doi:10.​1037/​a0010781.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.880.PubMed Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​31.​5.​880.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Getting a tool gives wings: overestimation of tool-related benefits in a motor imagery task and a decision task
Auteurs
François Osiurak
Nicolas Morgado
Guillaume T. Vallet
Marion Drot
Richard Palluel-Germain
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0485-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2014

Psychological Research 1/2014 Naar de uitgave